SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.38 número4Determinación de insulina en bolsas multicapa de nutrición parenteralDeterminantes de la salud en la adolescencia: capacidad cardiorrespiratoria y composición corporal índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Revista

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • En proceso de indezaciónCitado por Google
  • No hay articulos similaresSimilares en SciELO
  • En proceso de indezaciónSimilares en Google

Compartir


Nutrición Hospitalaria

versión On-line ISSN 1699-5198versión impresa ISSN 0212-1611

Nutr. Hosp. vol.38 no.4 Madrid jul./ago. 2021  Epub 20-Sep-2021

https://dx.doi.org/10.20960/nh.03474 

ORIGINAL PAPERS

Effect of overweight and obesity on cognitive function in children from 8 to 12 years of age: a descriptive study with a cross-sectional design

Efecto del sobrepeso y la obesidad sobre la función cognitiva en niños de 8 a 12 años de edad: estudio descriptivo con diseño transversal

Adriana Fabiola García-Martínez1  , Martha Patricia Hernández-Valdez1  , Arnulfo L'Gamiz-Matuk1  , Diego Incontri-Abraham1  , Antonio Ibarra1 

1Centro de Investigación en Ciencias de la Salud (CICSA). Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud. Universidad Anáhuac México, Campus Norte. Huixquilucan, Edo. de México. Mexico

Abstract

Introduction:

overweight and obesity in childhood and adolescence have progressively increased in recent years. In addition to known comorbidities, obesity has been related to poor school performance at all ages, and is associated with cognitive impairment.

Objective:

to determine the difference in cognitive function between children from 8 to 12 years of age with normal weight, overweight, and obesity.

Material and methods:

an observational, cross-sectional study was carried out in 46 children from 8 to 12 years of age. Children were classified into 3 groups: normal-weight, overweight, and obese. Subsequently, cognitive function tests were performed.

Results:

the majority of obese children presented cognitive impairment (63 %; p = 0.02), with a greater degree of impairment compared to that observed in the other groups (80 %; p < 0.05). On the other hand, it was observed that children with overweight still have the possibility of avoiding the development of cognitive impairment if they change their habits, since the results of this group were similar to those found in the normal-weight group.

Conclusions:

we found a significant increase not only in cognitive impairment, but also in its degree of severity in obese children as compared to those with overweight or normal weight.

Keywords: Childhood obesity; Overweight; Cognitive impairment; Cognitive function

Resumen

Introducción:

el sobrepeso y la obesidad en la infancia y la adolescencia se han incrementado progresivamente durante los últimos años. Además de las comorbilidades conocidas, la obesidad se ha relacionado con un bajo rendimiento escolar en todas las edades, asociándose a alteraciones cognitivas.

Objetivo:

determinar la diferencia que existe en la función cognitiva de unos niños de 8 a 12 años con normopeso, sobrepeso u obesidad.

Material y métodos:

se realizó un estudio observacional y transversal en 46 niños de 8 a 12 años. Los niños se clasificaron en 3 grupos: normopeso, sobrepeso y obesidad. Posteriormente se realizaron pruebas de función cognitiva.

Resultados:

la mayoría de los niños con obesidad presentaron deterioro cognitivo (63 %; p = 0.02)), con mayor grado de deterioro en comparación con el observado en los demás grupos (80 %; p < 0.05). Por otro lado se observó que los niños con sobrepeso aún tienen posibilidad de evitar el desarrollo del padecimiento si corrigen sus hábitos, ya que los resultados de este grupo fueron similares a los del grupo con normopeso.

Conclusiones:

encontramos un incremento significativo no solo del déficit cognitivo sino también del grado de severidad de este en los niños obesos en comparación con aquellos con sobrepeso o normopeso.

Palabras clave: Obesidad infantil; Sobrepeso; Deterioro cognitivo; Función cognitiva

INTRODUCTION

The incidence and prevalence of overweight and obesity have progressively increased in recent years, reaching values of 10 % to 30 % in childhood, and 30 % to 40 % in adolescence (1,2). These health problems have been linked to various comorbidities such as metabolic syndrome, type-2 diabetes, high blood pressure, cardiovascular disease, brain atrophy, and even cognitive disorders. At the cognitive level, obesity has been linked to deficits in school performance, interfering with cognitive functions such as memory, attention, and verbal and motor skills at all ages. These deficits may be due to the fact that excess body fat causes various alterations: insulin and leptin resistance, reduction of anti-inflammatory mediators, and a significant increase in pro-inflammatory mediators, among others (3-7). The inflammatory phenomenon observed in patients with excess fatty tissue is capable of significantly promoting inflammation of the neural tissue (neuroinflammation), and therefore alterations in neural function (2,8). Several studies have detected a reduction in the performance of executive function and verbal memory in adults with a high body mass index (BMI), compared to those who have a BMI within normal values (9-11). Concerning the pediatric population, evidence shows conflicting results. In a case-control study, it was reported that obese children older than 12 years exhibited a decrease in cognitive flexibility, motion ability, and attention capacity when compared to the group with normal weight (12). Other authors have reported that there is no relationship between BMI and cognitive tests in children and adults (13); however, other studies have shown that obese children exhibit decreased memory, abstract reasoning, verbal skills, and executive function (14,15).

In the present study we aimed to determine the difference that exists in the cognitive function of children from 8 to 12 years of age with normal weight, overweight or obesity.

MATERIAL AND METHODS

STUDY DESIGN

An observational, analytical, cross-sectional study was carried out in 46 children (16 males and 30 females) between 8 and 12 years of age from a public school in the State of Mexico. A nutritional assessment was performed, and children were classified into three study groups: 1) normal-weight children (n = 17); 2) overweight children (n = 13); 3) obese children (n = 16). Table I shows the inclusion criteria for each group. After the nutritional assessment, four cognitive function tests were performed, which are described below.

Table I.  Study groups 

BMI: body mass index; SD: standard deviation.

As this investigation was an exploratory study, sample size was determined by the feasibility of recruitment. A sample of 13 children per group was established considering that the number of children per group at school—with the criteria required for the study (overweight and obese)—fluctuated between 13 and 18 children. This sample size allowed the detection of an effect size of 0.1 or larger. In order to reach the established sample, we recruited 17 children per group according to the inclusion criteria for each group (Table I). From these children, four from the overweight and one child from the obese group were excluded from the study, as they did not complete the cognitive tests.

ETHICAL CONSIDERATIONS

This investigation was carried out under the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, the regulations of the General Health Law on Health Research Matters, and the Official Mexican Standard NOM-012-SSA3-2012. A letter of informed consent was obtained from family members and a letter of assent was obtained from each child included in the study. Patient records were always kept anonymous, and confidentiality was guaranteed for each child by providing a consecutive number on confidential files (16). All procedures were approved by the Research Committee at the Faculty of Health Sciences, Universidad Anáhuac México Campus Norte with the number 2015-08.

OVERWEIGHT AND OBESITY DIAGNOSIS

The nutritional diagnosis was made by nutritionists taking into account both anthropometric and clinical indicators. The standardization process was performed with prior training of personnel in the correct use of anthropometric equipment.

Anthropometric assessment

Weight (kg), height (cm), arm circumference (cm), tricipital skinfold (mm), and waist circumference (cm) were measured using the indicators of percentage of height for age, BMI (kg/m2), and percentage of body fat. Height for age and BMI were measured to rule out chronic and acute malnutrition, respectively. Triceps skinfold and arm circumference were measured to assess body fat percentage using Frisancho's formula (2).

The following measurement instruments were used for anthropometric assessment: scale with stadiometer (SECA), fiberglass measuring tape (SECA), and plicometer (Harpenden). A trained clinical nutritionist measured all parameters.

Clinical assessment

During the clinical assessment, it was contemplated to rule out any additional pathology that required the exclusion of the subject from the study.

COGNITIVE FUNCTION ASSESSMENT

Two tests were carried out to determine cognitive impairment (CI). Two other tests were performed to define the degree of cognitive ability deficiency. All tests were applied and evaluated by an expert psychologist in cognitive function.

Karen Machover projective test

This test consists of the child drawing on a blank paper a person and writing a short story about the person drawn. For the interpretation, each part of the body, the dimensions of the parts of the body, the omission of a line, and other aspects are evaluated. This allows obtaining data on psychomotor disorders or cognitive deficiency. The suspicion of cognitive function impairment is based on the type of drawing that the child makes since, depending on age, the child must have certain motor, perceptual, and visual abilities. If the drawing shows characteristics that represent a lower chronological age (disproportionate dimensions, lack of congruence between the parts that make up the drawing, etc.), it would indicate potential cognitive impairment (17-21).

Projective test: draw a person in the rain

This test is a complement to the "Karen Machover projective test"; however, it has an important difference: there is a stress factor, a person in the rain. The interpretation of this test is very similar to that of Karen Machover; however, it also takes into account the way the child behaves with the presence of the stress factor. The latter could be observed with the presence of the following objects in the drawing: umbrellas, clouds, lightning, facial expression, among others (18-22).

This test defines the existence or not of cognitive impairment. The results are obtained in the same way as those from the "Karen Machover projective test".

Bender-Gestalt test

This test consists of asking the child to draw 9 white cards with 9 different figures. The interpretation is performed according to the Koppitz scoring system, which gives a score that represents the cognitive age of the child. Shape distortion, rotation, omissions, sequence, overlay of figures, presence of circles or stripes instead of dots, wavy lines, shape of circles, wrong number of dots, presence of angles in curves, omission or addition of angles and boxes, among others, are items taken into account to provide a score for the child. The higher the score, the lower the cognitive age. A score of 1 represents a cognitive age of 10 to 12 years while a score greater than 13 represents a cognitive age of 5 years. Subsequently, cognitive age is compared to the chronological age of the child to establish the degree of cognitive development (23).

Comparison of cognitive and chronological age:

  • - From three to more years less: Neurological damage.

  • - Two years less: Medium cognitive ability deficiency.

  • - One year less: Low cognitive ability deficiency.

  • - Equivalence in both ages: Average cognitive ability.

  • - From one to two years more: Above-average cognitive ability.

André Rey for children test

This test consists of copying and later reproducing by memory a complex geometric drawing. The figures consist of a series of geometric elements that are identified separately. The results are classified according to the figures that are reproduced correctly (or according to the deformation of the figures) and the time required to carry them out. The results are then classified by percentiles according to the age of the patient. A percentile < 15 indicates medium cognitive ability deficiency; of 15-25, low cognitive-ability deficiency; of 25-75, average cognitive ability; and > 75, above-average cognitive ability (24).

STATISTICAL ANALYSIS

The results of the study were analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS). The statistical analysis was performed using the proportion z-Test. This test compares two proportions to find differences between them. In this study, we first compared, in the same group, the proportion of patients versus those without cognitive impairment. Afterwards, inter-group comparisons of patients with cognitive impairment were performed. Statistical significance was established at p ≤ 0.05.

RESULTS

The study was carried out in 46 children, 16 males and 30 females, with an average age of 10 years and 7 months, and a range of 8 to 12 years. Table II shows the demographic characteristics of the patients in the study.

Table II.  Demographic characteristics of the patients in the study 

AC: arm circumference; TSF: tricipital skinfold; WC: waist circumference; ZBMI: z-score of body mass index.

The results of each cognitive test are shown in Table III. This table also specifies how the cognitive status was classified.

Table III.  Result of cognitive evaluations 

Nw: normal-weight; Ow: overweight; Ob: obese.

Figure 1 shows that the majority of obese patients presented cognitive impairment (63 %), and this proportion was significantly higher than among those who did not show cognitive alterations in the same group (37 %; p = 0.012; proportion z-Test). This proportion was also significantly higher than the ones observed in the other groups (p = 0.02). In the group of overweight patients we found a very similar distribution to that presented in the normal-weight group. In both groups the proportion of patients without cognitive impairment was high. However, only in the case of the normal-weight group the difference between patients with and without cognitive impairment was significant (overweight group: p = 0.370; normal-weight group: p = 0.015; proportion z-Test).

Figure 1.  Cognitive impairment in the study groups. A higher proportion of obese children presented cognitive impairment. The bars represent the percentage of the total children in each group. *p = 0.015; **p = 0.020; ***p = 0.012. CI: cognitive impairment. 

With the objective of analyzing the degree of cognitive development, according to nutritional status, we determined the average cognitive ability in each group. According to this evaluation the cognitive ability deficiency could be low, medium or high (23,24). Figure 2 shows that a significant percentage of obese patients (80 %) presented medium cognitive deficiency (p = 0.019; proportion z-Test). This proportion was significantly higher than in the other groups (p < 0.05). In the case of overweight and normal-weight patients, the proportions were very similar between them and there was no significant difference between low and medium impairment.

Figure 2.  Degree of cognitive ability deficiency in the study groups. A majority of obese children presented medium cognitive ability deficiency. The bars represent the percentage of the total number of children with cognitive impairment in each group. *p = 0.019; **p < 0.05. CI: cognitive impairment. 

With the objective of analyzing whether excess body weight, that is, that of overweight and obese patients (OwOb), significantly changed the proportion of patients with cognitive impairment, the differences in cognitive function between patients with normal weight and OwOb were also evaluated. Figure 3A shows how the cognitive function behaves in the OwOb group and in the normal-weight group. The OwOb group exhibited a significant increase in the percentage of patients with cognitive impairment (p = 0.020; proportion z-Test). This proportion was significantly higher than the one presented by the normal-weight group (p = 0.015). On the other hand, the normal-weight group presented a significantly lower percentage of patients with cognitive impairment (p = 0.010).

Figure 3 A. Cognitive impairment in OwOb children. When analyzing OwOb children together, a higher proportion of children with cognitive impairment is still observed. The bars represent the percentage of the total children in each group. *p = 0.010; **p = 0.020; ***p = 0.015. CI: cognitive impairment; OwOb: overweight and obese. B. Degree of cognitive-ability deficiency in OwOb children. A majority of OwOb children presented medium cognitive deficiency. The bars represent the percentage of the total number of children with cognitive impairment in the two groups. *p = 0.010; **p < 0.05. CI: cognitive impairment; OwOb: overweight or obese. 

When evaluating the degree of cognitive ability deficiency in the OwOb group, it can be seen in figure 3B that a higher proportion of OwOb patients exhibited a medium cognitive-ability deficiency level compared to those with low cognitive impairment (p = 0.010; proportion z-Test). This proportion with medium cognitive deficiency was also significantly higher than among patients with normal weight (p < 0.05). Regarding the normal-weight group, there was no significant difference in average of cognitive ability (p = 0.324).

DISCUSSION

In the present study, it was found that a higher proportion of obese children tend to present significant cognitive impairment, and that this group has a higher degree of severity when compared to both the overweight and normal-weight groups.

Our results agree with previous studies that had reported a significant effect of obesity on cognitive function in children and adolescents (25-28).

In the present study we found a significant increase not only in cognitive impairment but also in its degree of severity among obese patients. Similarly, when we analyzed OwOb patients together, a significant increase in the proportion of patients with cognitive impairment was observed. These results open the possibility that excess body weight could make a significant difference in the cognitive function of children, generating a long-term negative effect. Our work also suggests that children who are only overweight still have the possibility of avoiding the development of cognitive impairment if they change their habits, since in the present study the results of these children were very similar to those found for normal-weight children.

The possible inducing mechanisms of cognitive impairment in obese patients are diverse, including alterations in glycoregulation, reduction of neurotrophin levels, neuroinflammation, and structural alterations of the blood-brain barrier. Cytokines appear to have an important role in obesity and neurodegeneration (25,29).

Recent research has suggested that, under conditions of obesity, in adipose tissue there is an increase in the release of adipocytokines that are highly inflammatory, such as leptin, tumor necrosis factor alpha (TNF-alpha) and interleukin 6 (IL-6), among others (30).

These alterations lead to a chronic inflammatory state with repercussions at both the local and systemic levels, which causes alterations in the brain, especially in the hypothalamus. This neuroinflammation is likely causing synaptic remodeling and neurodegeneration in the hypothalamus, thereby altering the internal and output circuits to other regions of the brain. The final result is a disruption of cognitive function (31).

Although the mentioned mechanisms remain theories, the increase in IL-6 has been shown to cause long-term memory impairments. Similarly, insulin resistance seems to play an extremely important role in cognition, since excess abdominal adiposity and insulin resistance have been shown to cause cognitive impairment in patients of all ages (29,32-40).

CONCLUSIONS

In the present study it is observed that cognitive impairment is greater in obese children from 8 to 12 years of age when compared to those who are only in the overweight or normal-weight groups. Cognitive processes can be influenced by chronic inflammation as induced by excess body weight.

REFERENCES

1. NORMA Oficial Mexicana NOM-008-SSA3-2010, Para el tratamiento integral del sobrepeso y la obesidad. [ Links ]

2. Encuesta Nacional de Salud y Nutrición. Resultados Nacionales;2016. [ Links ]

3. Pérez M. El adipocito como órgano endocrino. Rev Med 2007;15(2):225-42. [ Links ]

4. Guxens M, Mendez M, Julvez J, Plana E, Forns J, Basagaña X, et al. Cognitive Function and Overweight in Preschool Children. American Journal of Epidemiology 2009;170(4):438-46. DOI:10.1093/aje/kwp140 [ Links ]

5. Fotuhi M, Lubinski B. The Effects of Obesity on Brain Structure and Size. Practical Neurology 2013;20-34. [ Links ]

6. Jeong S, Nam H, Son M, Son E, Cho K. Interactive Effect of Obesity Indexes on Cognition. Dementia &Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 2005;19(2-3):91-6. DOI:10.1159/000082659 [ Links ]

7. Marcano Y, Torcat J, Ayala L, Verdi B, Lairet C, Maldonado M, et al. Funciones endocrinas del tejido adiposo. Rev. Vene Endocrinol Metabol 2006:4(1):15-21. [ Links ]

8. Frisancho R. New norms of upper limb fat and muscle areas for assessment of nutritional status. Am J Clin Nutr 1981;34(11):2540-5. DOI:10.1093/ajcn/34.11.2540 [ Links ]

9. Gunstad J, Paul RH, Cohen RA, Tate DF, Gordon E. Obesity is associated with memory deficits in young and middle-aged adults. Eating and Weight Disorders 2006;1:15-9. DOI:10.1007/BF03327747 [ Links ]

10. Gunstad J, Paul RH, Cohen RA, Tate DF, Spitznagel MB, Gordon E. Elevated body mass index is associated with executive dysfunction in otherwise healthy adults. Compr Psychiatry 2007;1:57-61. DOI:10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.05.001 [ Links ]

11. Elias MF, Elias PK, Sullivan LM, Wolf PA, D'Agostino RB. Lower cognitive function in the presence of obesity and hypertension:the Framingham heart study. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2003;2:260-8. DOI:10.1038/sj.ijo.802225 [ Links ]

12. Cserjesia R, Molnár D, Luminet O, Lénárd L. Is there any relationship between obesity and mental flexibility in children? Appetite 2007;675-8. DOI:10.1016/j.appet.2007.04.001 [ Links ]

13. Gunstad J, Spitznagel MB, RH Paul, RA Cohen R, M Kohn, Luyster FA, et al. Body mass index and neuropsychological function in healthy children and adolescents. Appetite 2008;50(2-3):246-51. DOI:10.1016/j.appet.2007.07.008 [ Links ]

14. Portillo-Reyes V, Puente AE, Pérez-García M, Loya-Méndez Y. Deterioro Neuropsicológico en Niños Mexicanos con Estados Nutricionales Alterados:Desnutrición Leve a Moderada vs. Obesidad. Revista Neuropsicología, Neuropsiquiatría y Neurociencias 2011;11:133-46. [ Links ]

15. Guxens M, Méndez MA, Julvez J, Plana E, Forns J, Basagaña X, et al. Cognitive functionand overweight in preschool children. American Journal of epidemiology 2009;170:438-46. DOI:10.1093/aje/kwp140 [ Links ]

16. Declaración de Helsinki de la Asociación Médica Mundial. Principios éticos para las investigaciones médicas en seres humanos. Asamblea General;2013. [ Links ]

17. Portuondo J. La figura humana:Test proyectivo de Karen Machover;2007. [ Links ]

18. Hogan TP. Pruebas psicológicas:una introducción práctica. Editorial El Manual Moderno;2015. [ Links ]

19. Muñoz JM. La evaluación psicológica forense del daño psíquico:propuesta de un protocolo de actuación pericial. Anuario de psicología jurídica 2013;23(1):61-9. DOI:10.5093/aj2013a10 [ Links ]

20. Petroselli A. Un enfoque de trabajo en psicología clínica infantil:análisis de un caso. Revista de Psicología. Tercera época 1983;9. [ Links ]

21. Zusman L. Anorexia nervosa:un estudio de casos. Revista de Psicología 2013;8(2):117-31. DOI:10.18800/psico.199002.002 [ Links ]

22. Querol SM, Chaves Paz MI. Test de la persona bajo la lluvia. Adaptación y aplicación. Ed. Lugar 2ª ed. Buenos aires;2005. [ Links ]

23. Test gestáltico Bender modificado y VMI-4:comparación de la validez incremental. Psicoperspecivas 2013;12(1):183-204. DOI:10.5027/psicoperspectivas-Vol12-Issue1-fulltext-201 [ Links ]

24. Rey A. Rey:Test de copia y de reproducción de memoria de figuras geométricas complejas. Madrid:TEA ediciones;1997. [ Links ]

25. Liang J. Matheson BE, Kaye, WH, Boutelle KN. Neurocognitive correlates of obesity and obesity-related behaviors in children and adolescents. International Journal of Obesity 2014;38(4):494-506. DOI:10.1038/ijo.2013.142 [ Links ]

26. Cserjesia R, Molnár D, Luminet O, Lénárd L. Is there any relationship between obesity and mental flexibility in children? Appetite 2007;675-8. DOI:10.1016/j.appet.2007.04.001 [ Links ]

27. Reinert K, Po'e, EK, Barkin SL. The Relationship between Executive Function and Obesity in Children and Adolescents:A Systematic Literature Review. Journal of Obesity 2013. DOI:10.1155/2013/820956 [ Links ]

28. Davis CL, Cooper S. Fitness, fatness, cognition, behavior, and academic achievement among overweight children:Do cross-sectional associations correspond to exercise trial outcomes? Preventive Medicine 2011;52(1):S65-9. DOI:10.1016/j.ypmed.2011.01.020 [ Links ]

29. Khan NA, Raine LB, Donovan SM, Hillman CH. The cognitive implications of obesity and nutrition in childhood. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 2014;79(4)51-70. DOI:10.1111/mono.12130 [ Links ]

30. Izaola O, Luis D, Sajoux I, Domingo JC, Vidal M. Inflamación y obesidad (lipoinflamación). Nutr Hosp 2015;31(6):2352-8. [ Links ]

31. Miller AA, Spencer SJ. Obesity and Neuroinflammation:A Pathway to Cognitive Impairment. Brain Behav Immun 2014;42:10-21. DOI:10.1016/j.bbi.2014.04.001 [ Links ]

32. Cuartas JM. Cognición e inflamación:"El papel de las citoquinas en el desempeño cognitivo". Int J Psychol Res 2014;7(2):8-10. [ Links ]

33. Arisi GM. Nervous and immune systems signals and connections:Cytokines in hippocampus physiology and pathology. Epilepsy Behav 2014;38:43-7. DOI:10.1016/j.yebeh.2014.01.017 [ Links ]

34. Ferrer B. Influencia de la citoquina interleuquina 6 (IL-6) adipocitaria y muscular en el control del metabolismo. Universidad Autónoma de Barcelona. Tesis Doctoral;2013. [ Links ]

35. Raji CA, Ho AJ, Parikshak NN, Becker JT, Lopez OL, Kuller LH, et al. Brain structure and obesity. Hum Brain Mapp 2010;31(3):353-64. [ Links ]

36. Ramírez Alvarado MM, Sánchez Roitz C. El factor de necrosis tumoral-α, la resistencia a la insulina, el metabolismo de lipoproteínas y la obesidad en humanos. Nutr Hosp 2012;27(6):1751-7. [ Links ]

37. Blancas-Flores G, Almanza-Pérez JC, López-Roa RI, Alarcón-Aguilar FJ, García-Macedo R, Cruz M. La obesidad como un proceso inflamatorio. Bol Med Hosp Infant Mex 2010;67(2):88-97. [ Links ]

38. Argente J. Nuevas funciones neuroendocrinas de la leptina. Rev Esp Endocrinol Pediatr 2014;5. [ Links ]

39. Whitmer RA, Gustafson DR, Barrett-Connor E, Haan MN, Gunderson EP, Yaffe K. Central obesity and increased risk of dementia more than three decades later. Neurology 2008;71(14):1057-64. DOI:10.1212/01.wnl.0000306313.89165.ef [ Links ]

40. Soreca I, Rosano C, Jennings JR, Sheu LK, Kuller LH, Matthews KA, et al. Gain in adiposity across 15 years is associated with reduced gray matter volume in healthy women. Psychosom Med 2009;71(5):485-90. DOI:10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181a5429d [ Links ]

Protection of human and animal subjects:the authors declare that no experiments were performed on humans or animals for this study.

Confidentiality of data:the authors declare that they have followed the protocols of their work center on the publication of patient data.

Right to privacy and informed consent:the authors have obtained a written informed consent of all the patients or subjects mentioned in the article. The corresponding author is in possession of this document.

Funding source:this work was financed by the Universidad Anáhuac México.

Received: December 09, 2020; Accepted: May 03, 2021

Correspondence: Antonio Ibarra. Centro de Investigación en Ciencias de la Salud. Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud. Universidad Anáhuac México Campus Norte. Av. Universidad Anáhuac nº 46, Col. Lomas Anáhuac. C.P. 52786, Huixquilucan, Estado de México. México e-mail: jose.ibarra@anahuac.mx

Conflicts of interest:

the authors declare no competing interests. The contents of this publication are solely the responsibility of the authors.

Creative Commons License This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License