SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.32 número3El poder de los extravertidos: evaluando la regulación de estados de ánimo positivos y negativosEficacia de una intervención en psicología positiva combinada con terapia cognitivo conductual en estudiantes universitarios índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Revista

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • En proceso de indezaciónCitado por Google
  • No hay articulos similaresSimilares en SciELO
  • En proceso de indezaciónSimilares en Google

Compartir


Anales de Psicología

versión On-line ISSN 1695-2294versión impresa ISSN 0212-9728

Anal. Psicol. vol.32 no.3 Murcia oct. 2016

https://dx.doi.org/10.6018/analesps.32.3.261651 

 

 

Shared flow and positive collective gatherings

Flujo Compartido y Reuniones Colectivas Positivas

 

 

Larraitz Zumeta1, Nekane Basabe1, Anna Wlodarczyk1, 2, Magdalena Bobowik1 and Darío Páez1, 2

1 University of the Basque Country (Spain).
2 University of Santiago de Chile (Chile).

This work was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation under Grant PSI2014-51923-P; the University of the Basque Country under Grant IT-666-13 and Grant UFI 11/04]; Basque Governments Research Personnel Education and Training Program scholarship granted to Larraitz Zumeta [PRE_2013_1_738] and Basque Government's Doctoral Research Staff Improvement Program post-doctoral funding awarded by to Anna Wlodarczyk [POS_2015_1_0075].

Correspondence

 

 


ABSTRACT

Collective gatherings or rituals promote optimal experiences in socially acceptable circumstances. Few studies have empirically examined the experience of flow shared by a group in collective situations. The present research examined the multi-dimensional structure of shared flow experience and its role in explaining positive effects of participation in collective ritualized gatherings on personal wellbeing and social cohesion. In this longitudinal study (N = 550) participants of a local festival celebrated in San Sebastian (tamborrada) responded to an online questionnaire at three different times. Confirmatory factor analyses supported a structure composed of nine first-order factors and one second-order factor with a 27-item version of the scale. Further, structural equation modeling analyses controlling for the pre-event scores showed indirect effects of participation in tamborrada through shared flow on happiness, collective efficacy, identity fusion, and social integration. This research concludes that positive collective gatherings stimulate shared flow experiences and thus promote personal wellbeing and social cohesion. We discuss both the implications of these results and the utility of the Shared Flow Scale in positive psychology research.

Key words: shared flow; collective gatherings; positive psychology; wellbeing; collective efficacy; identity fusion.


RESUMEN

Las reuniones colectivas o rituales promueven experiencias óptimas en circunstancias socialmente aceptables. Pocos estudios han examinado empíricamente la experiencia de flujo compartido por un grupo en situaciones colectivas. El presente estudio examinó la estructura multidimensional de la experiencia de flujo compartido y su papel en la explicación de los efectos positivos de la participación en reuniones colectivas ritualizadas sobre el bienestar personal y la cohesión social. En este estudio longitudinal (N = 550) los y las participantes de un festival local celebrado en San Sebastián (tamborrada) respondieron a cuestionarios online en tres momentos diferentes. El análisis factorial confirmatorio apoyó una estructura compuesta por nueve factores de primer orden y un factor de segundo orden en una versión de la escala de 27 ítems. Además, los análisis de modelos estructurales controlando las puntuaciones previas al evento mostraron efectos indirectos de la participación en la tamborrada a través de flujo compartido en el bienestar, en la eficacia colectiva, en la fusión de identidad y en la integración social. Esta investigación concluye que las reuniones colectivas positivas estimulan las experiencias de flujo compartido y por lo tanto, promueven el bienestar personal y la cohesión social. Se discuten las implicaciones de estos resultados y la utilidad de la Escala de Flujo Compartido en la investigación en psicología positiva.

Palabras clave: flujo compartido; reuniones colectivas; psicología positiva; bienestar; eficacia colectiva; fusión de identidad.


 

Shared Flow and Positive Collective Gatherings

Over the last few years the interest in studying collective emotions and participation in collective emotional gatherings has significantly increased (Collins, 2004; von Scheve & Ismer, 2013). Growing empirical evidence confirms that participation in collective emotional gatherings might have major beneficial effects. At the individual level, it increases wellbeing and happiness; at the intra-group level, it boosts collective efficacy and collective self-esteem; at the social level, it enhances social cohesion and integration even with a broader group; and finally, at the symbolic level, it affects positive and self-transcendence beliefs about the benevolence of people and world in general (Pâez, Rimé, Basabe, Wlodarczyk, & Zumeta, 2015; Rimé, Pâez, Basabe, & Martinez, 2010; Whitehouse & Lanman, 2014; Zumeta, Basabe, Telletxea, Amutio, & Bobowik, in press).

Simultaneously, studies have revealed that optimal experiences are particularly intense and enjoyable when they are performed collectively (Walker, 2010). In these situations, participants transcend their ego and get involved in a more complex action system that activates collective emotions (Csikszentmihâlyi, 1990). In this same line, there is an increasing interest in the study of flow experiences from a collective perspective (Delle Fave, Massimini, & Bassi, 2013; Salanova, Rodriguez-Sânchez, Schaufeli, & Cifre, 2014; Zumeta, Oriol, Telletxea, Amutio, & Basabe, 2016). Shared emotional and behavioral processes are an important and exciting area of research in psychology and social sciences in general, where classic ideas like Durkheim's (1912) collective effervescence and emotional communion are examined with advanced research tools and more sophisticated macro psychological approaches (see von Scheve & Salmela, 2014 for a review of the collective emotions).

The present study integrates research on collective optimal experiences and the positive effects of participation in collective emotional gatherings. We aim to analyze shared flow resulting from the exposure to an intense social interaction experienced during ritualized gatherings, as well as its role in explaining effects of participation in such activities on personal and collective outcomes. To our knowledge, this is the first study that examines how collective emotional gatherings activate optimal experience, not only at the personal level, but also as a state of synchronized collective optimal experience, i.e., shared flow. Further, the present investigation examines shared optimal experience in the context of real-life collective emotional gatherings, while building on previous mostly experimental research in laboratory settings (see Pâez, et al., 2015, study 4 for an experimental induction of flow).

Collective Gatherings, Collective Effervescence and Shared Flow

The concept of flow has usually been studied from the individual perspective. In these terms, flow is a positive emotional state that occurs when a person is fully immersed and absorbed in an activity. It involves an optimal psychological state in which individuals get completely abstracted in the execution of the task, reaching a level of optimum performance. Flow is composed of nine dimensions divided into three interconnected stages: antecedents, experiences and effects (Jackson & Csíkszentmihályi, 2002). The antecedents of an optimal experience are clear goals (1), balance between challenge and individual's abilities (2), and a direct feedback (3). The outcomes that follow an experience of flow are the concentration on the task (4), the merging of action and awareness (5), and the consequent sense of control over the activity (6). Lastly, the effects are the loss of selfconsciousness (7), the distortion of time (8) and the autotelic experience (9) (Schiepe-Tiska & Engeser, 2012). Existing research has confirmed the validity of a nine-dimensional model of individual flow (Delle Fave & Bassi, 2009; García Calvo, Jiménez, Santos-Rosa, Reina, & Cervelló, 2008; Jackson & Eklund, 2002). Importantly, flow is a multifaceted experience, and according to the existing evidence distinct facets of flow are correlated. In this regard, the current research aims to determine constituent components of flow and its relationships (Engeser & Schiepe-Tiska, 2012).

On the other hand, Csíkszentmihályi (1990, p. 432) stated that "participation in collective emotional gatherings can be seen as an opportunity to share experiences within a society". In this sense, Durkheim's concept of collective effervescence and the notion of flow are convergent (Csíkszent-mihályi, 1990). Participants in collective gatherings and rituals that could afford flow usually display shared and focused attention, as well as coordinated collective behaviors (shared gestures, shared movements, moving and marching together) in such a way that every participant's mind, voice and body becomes attuned to the shared emotional state in the group and thus enacts behavioral synchrony. These preceding elements concur in stimulating participants' emotional arousal in such a way that they all experience and enact similar emotional states. Since emotions are easily mirrored, shared and spread among individuals, a situation of emotional synchrony develops, intensifying emotions and perceptions of similarity and unity (Collins, 2004, Páez et al., 2015; Rossano, 2012; Von Scheve & Ismer, 2013). What is central here is that individuals are gathered together, perceiving common feelings that are expressed through joint events. Group members need to be in communion, i.e., united in the same mind and in the same action (Páez et al., 2015).

Shared Flow, Positive Affect and Well-being

A cross-cultural study (Delle Fave et al., 2013) suggests that flow is a universally shared experience. The optimal experiences are common: 85% of individuals report experiencing flow in one or more activities of daily life; of those, 54% chose structured sports activities, 52% physical activities, whereas 47%hobbies and games. Importantly, these states are expected to reinforce positive emotions and well-being. For instance, positive psychology scholars propose that optimal experience promotes eudaimonic well-being because individuals select and cultivate activities and relationships that contribute to their individual development and quality of life (Delle Fave & Bassi, 2009).

Some empirical evidence supports these ideas. With respect to the importance of attention and behavior synchrony, interviews with athletes revealed that in team sports, unison movements and shared focus of attention are critical to the experience of flow (Schiepe-Tiska & Engeser, 2012). As for the emotional intensity, other studies show that social flow, in interaction with others, provides more positive affect and enjoyment than solitary flow, presumably due to the fact that the emotions produced by interactive flow are amplified by social sharing (Mesurado, 2009; Walker, 2010).

Shared Flow, Collective Efficacy, and Group Identity

However, above positive outcomes for personal wellbeing, shared flow is primarily expected to strengthen group outcomes. One important positive group outcome of the experience of shared flow may be increased perception of group efficacy.

Collective flow occurs when a group is performing at the peak of its abilities (Sawyer, 2003) and implies a clear collective goal involving a challenge to the group in balance with the individual abilities as well as direct feedback in respect to the team behavior. Subsequently, this group experience that follows are the concentration combined outcome of the group task and performance, a sense of merging of action and awareness with the collective behavior, and the consequent sense control over the team activity.

Synchronization of activity among group members achieved in the state of collective flow may also contribute to higher perceived group efficacy. In the state of shared flow, all members of the group experience the same sensation of being absorbed by the activity, while the synchrony of movements and shared emotions increase the perceived collective efficacy. According to the sport psychology literature, synchronized movements and shared focus on attention among peers are essential elements of flow experience in the competition (Schiepe-Tiska & Engeser, 2012). At the same time, experimental results have found greater group cooperation when synchrony and shared intentionality were combined (Reddish, Fischer, & Bulbulia, 2013). Furthermore, several studies showed that while experiencing flow in sports people tended to enjoy more, obtain more satisfaction, and display more concentration and control over their performance (Csíkszentmihályi, 1990; Jackson & Csikszentmihâlyi, 2002; Moradi, Nima, Rapp Ricciardi, Archer, & García, 2014). In consequence, flow was shown to have very high impact on success (Nicholls, Polman, & Holt, 2005); affecting not only perceived collective efficacy but also team performance.

An empirical study found that shared flow is experienced when both collective challenges and collective skills are high, and also as a result of high collective efficacy beliefs as means to overcome potential obstacles. Moreover, beliefs in collective efficacy predict collective flow over time, both being related reciprocally (Salanova et al., 2014).

Finally, in parallel to effects on group efficacy, shared experience of flow is expected to strengthen group identity. Csikszentmihâlyi (1990) states that loss of self-consciousness allows individuals to momentarily give up their normal identities and perform different roles. Similarly, the experience of collective flow involves a loss of consciousness of the "self', a perception of fusion with other participants (Walker, 2010). In these sense, Pâez et al. (2015) revealed that collective activities also generate an identity fusion with the group that is absent in individual activities.

Thus, we argue that collective optimal experiences, collective effervescence or emotional communion and behavioral and emotional synchronization are interdependent. During optimal experiences, particularly collective ones, participants transcend their ego while getting involved in a more complex action system (Csíkszentmihályi, 1990). In sum, shared flow during collective gatherings entail a number of positive psychosocial consequences among participants at individual level and also in terms of collective identities and social cohesion.

Overview, Aims, and Hypotheses of the Study

This study focuses on an important folk tradition in the city of San Sebastian-Donostia in the Spanish Basque Country. Annually, during a 24 hour-long celebration of St. Sebastian's Feast Day, the entire city is transformed with the sound of drums and barrels. Collective drum marches, parades accompanied by large groups of "drummers" march down the city streets. According to municipal records about 15,143 people distributed in 125 real-life folk groups marched at the time of this research.

The goal of this study was to test the structural validity of the Shared Flow Scale in the context of this social activity. We measured the nine dimensions of flow, using individuals as a unit of analysis, but with items that refer to group activity. First, we postulated a second-order factor of shared flow with the nine first order factors or flow components (Jackson & Eklund, 2002) (Hip.1). Second, we expected that shared flow during a collective gathering would predict an increase in personal well-being, collective efficacy, fusion of identity with the group, and social integration, after participation in the collective gathering (controlling for pretest scores of these criterion variables as baseline) in a quasi longitudinal study. Also, we argue that shared flow will mediate the effect of participation in folk festival on the increase in the previously mentioned personal and collective outcomes (Hlp.2).

 

Method

Participants

The convenience sample consisted of a total of 550 volunteers that completed the questionnaires at three time points (87% of all the recruited participants). The study participants were members of 52 different folk groups or tamborradas (CI = 95%, sigma = 1.96; Error = ± 4.10). Males accounted for 50.7% of the sample. Age ranged between 18 and 90 (M = 42.75 years, SD = 13.98), most of them (89.2%) were residents of San Sebastian while the rest declared to reside in the surroundings of the city. The 57% of the sample were married, 53% had children, whereas 73% were employed and 11% unemployed.

Procedure

Town Hall officers and coordinators of folkloric companies (one per each tamborrada group) were contacted in order to recruit a sample of volunteers for this study among participants in the tamborrada held on January 20th, 2013. Their participation in the study was voluntary, and each person selected for the sample was provided with relevant information about the purpose of the investigation. Email addresses of each participant were recorded and encrypted personal emails were used to collect data online (Survey Monkey®) at three measurement times. Four days before (T1) the tamborrada, and again four days after it (T3), participants completed identical questionnaires measuring personal well-being, collective efficacy, identity fusion with the group, and social integration. Those questionnaires were used to examine the outcomes of participation in the celebration and to monitor baseline scores. Also, on the afternoon following the tamborrada, all participants filled out a short questionnaire that assessed their level of involvement in the activity and their perception of experience of shared flow (T2).

Measures

Measurement during the event (T2)

Level of involvement. Participants were asked to evaluate their involvement in the social activity regarding five different aspects: importance, intensity, satisfaction, involvement and pride (Zumeta, et al., in press). For example: "How intense was your participation in the tamborrada? The rank of response was from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much). Cronbach's α was .91.

Shared flow. The scale measuring shared flow originally derived from the Spanish version of Jackson and Marsh's Dispositional Flow Scale (1996) and its adaptation by García Calvo et al., 2008. This scale was originally developed by Zumeta et al. 2013, was applied by Páez et al., 2015 and Zumeta et al., 2016. The scale comprises 27 items distributed across nine dimensions: (1) Balance between challenge and skill; (2) Clear proximal goals; (3) Unambiguous and direct feedback; (4) Action-awareness merging; (5) Focused concentration on the current activity; (6) Sense of control over one's actions; (7) Loss of self-consciousness; (8) Loss of time awareness or time acceleration; (9) Autotelic experience. The rank of response was from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much). Cronbach's α for total score was .95.

Pre-event (T1) and post-event (T3) measures

Personal well-being. The Pemberton Happiness Index (PHI, Vázquez & Hervás, 2012) contained 10 items, measured against an 11-point scale ranging from 0 (strongly disagree) to 10 (strongly agree) to calculate general, eudaimonic, hedonic, and social well-being (e.g. "I have done something, I really enjoyed"), yielding a single well-being index. Cronbach's alpha coefficients were excellent, with α .93 for pre-tamborrada (T1) and α .94 for post-tamborrada measurements (T3).

Collective efficacy. A short 4-item version was adapted from the CEQS-Collective Efficacy Questionnaire for Sports (Martínez, Guillén, & Feltz, 2011), to assess the respondent's perception of group efficacy (e.g. "Showed more abilities than other groups"). The items were placed on an 11-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 10 (very much). Reliability coefficients were .91 at the pre-event and .92 at the postevent evaluation.

Identity fusion. Fusion with the tamborrada group was measured using the 7-item verbal fusion scale (e.g., "I am one with my group") (Gómez et al., 2011). Respondents indicated the degree to which each statement reflected their relationship with their tamborrada group on scales ranging from 0 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). Cronbach's alpha coefficients were α = .93 for pre-tamborrada and α = .92 for post-tamborrada measurements.

Social Integration. The 10-item Feeling of Relatedness Scale developed by Richer and Vallerand (1998) (ESAS) assessed participants' perception of social integration (e.g. "In my relationships with my work colleagues, I feel supported"). The rank of responses varied from 0 (completely disagree) to 10 (completely agree). The scale yielded Cronbach's alphas of .96 and .98 in measurements before and after the tamborrada, respectively.

Statistical analysis

The shared flow scale was analyzed and descriptive statistics were reported. First, Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was used to contrast structure of shared flow using maximum likelihood estimation in Mplus 6.11 (Muthén & Muthén, 2010). To evaluate model fit, in addition to the chi-squared test, the following indexes were used: the CFI (Comparative Fit Index, values above .90 are considered acceptable), as well as the RMSEA (Root Mean Square Error of Approximation, values less than .07 indicate an adequate fit) (Chen, 2007; Steiger, 2007). Second, in order to test whether involvement in the activity explains the enhancement of different positive outcomes, and whether this effect is mediated by flow, we applied structural equation modeling (SEM) with ML estimation. We computed indirect effects (standardized estimates are presented). Considering the lack of multivariate normality, we applied the bootstrap method (Efron & Tibshirani, 1993). Standard errors and confidence intervals based on a bootstrap sampling distribution were calculated for each parameter or statistics. If the values of the estimated effect within the confidence interval include zero, this indicates a non-significant effect. In the presentation of the results, the standardized solution is shown. All the coefficients represented by continuous arrows in the graphs are statistically significant, while the dashed lines indicate effects that are not statistically significant for p < .05.

 

Results

Descriptive Analyses

Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics of the 27 items from the shared flow scale, including the means and standard deviation for each item. All mean scores were higher than 4.00 (on a scale from 1 to 7), which shows that participants experienced relatively high levels of shared flow during the activity. Items with higher means were those corresponding to the dimensions of balance between challenge and skill (SF9), clear goals (SF12) and autotelic experience (SF27). Consequently, items with lower mean scores corresponded to the dimensions of action-awareness merging (SF11, SF20) and temporary distortion (SF26).

Shared Flow Scale: Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Based on the theoretical and empirical work with individual flow scales, four alternative models were specified: (1) a one-factor baseline model; (2) a three first-order correlated factor model separating items measuring conditions for flow, the experience of flow and the consequences resulting from the experience; and (3) the a model composed of one second-order and nine first order-factors, where each firstorder factor was specified to load on a single second-order factor representing general concept of shared flow. Models were compared with each other in order to determine which provided a better fit to the data.

Table 2 presents the fit indices for the specified models. Chi-square difference tests and global fit indices indicated that the Model 3 (Nine factors and one second order factor) provided a better fit to the data than the single (Model 1), or three-factor (Model 2) models. Model 1 and 3 provided very poor fit to the data and the ACFI value was significant. In consequence, these models were rejected. On the other hand, all global fit indices for Model 3 were satisfactory (Hip. 1).

 

 

The standardized loadings for the higher order nine-factor model were high and statistically significant for all first-order factors, (Bs ranged between .67 and .90, ps < .001). The factorial scores of second-order factor on the nine latent factors or facets showed that the highest scores were obtained for the dimensions of clear goal, balance between challenge and skill, direct feedback, high concentration, strong sense of control, and autotelic experience. Nevertheless, distortion of time showed moderate factor score, and merging of action and loss of self-consciousness showed low scores.

Mediation Effects: the Role of Shared Flow in Explaining Effects of Participation in a Folk Festival on Personal and Group Outcomes

Table 3 shows descriptive statistics, means, and standard deviation for each of the study variables, as well as the bivariate correlations. Missing values for each variable reached values below 5%. As can be seen in Table 3, all variables correlated significantly and positively. The size of the correlation coefficients indicates the presence of medium positive associations between the studied variables. The rank of correlations fluctuates from the lowest correlation between level of involvement with well being, r = .29, p < .001, to the highest found between level of involvement and shared flow, r = .58, p < .001.

 

 

Further, we tested indirect effects of participation in the folk festival on personal and group outcomes after the event mediated by the experience of shared flow. Accordingly, we estimated four different empirical SEM models where the relationship between level of involvement and personal well being, collective efficacy, fusion of identity with the tamborrada team, and social integration after the event (i.e., controlling the pre-test scores), was hypothesized to be explained by shared flow

In the first model (Figure 1) we tested indirect effect of level of involvement on personal well-being via shared flow. The model provided a good fit χ2(551, 1252) = 3343.960, p < .001, CFI = .917, RMSEA = .055]. The relationship between level of involvement in tamborrada and the increase of well-being was fully mediated by the experience of shared flow during the event (B = .14, SE = .02, Est./S.E. = 5.98, p < .001, 99% CI [0.082, 0.205]) (Hip. 2).

The second model (Figure 2) tested whether shared flow mediated the effect of the involvement on collective efficacy. The model fit was satisfactory [χ2(551, 719) - 2083.063, p < .001, CFI - .917, RMSEA - .059], and confirmed that the effect of level of involvement on perception of collective efficacy was explained by the experience of shared flow (B.24, SE = .03, Est./S.E. = 6.95, p < .001, 99% CI [0.340, = 0.753]).

Subsequently, we tested a similar meditational model for identity fusion (Figure 3). Examination of this model showed adequate fit to the data [χ2(551, 968) = 2729.437, p < .001, CFI = .913, RMSEA = .057]. The results indicated that there was a direct significant and positive effect between the studied variables, and that the relationship between the level of involvement and the increase in identity fusion was mediated by the shared flow experience (B = .13, SE = .03, Est./S.E. = 4.57, p < .001, 99% CI [0.057, 0.205]).

 

 

 

The fourth model, tested the effects of the level of involvement on social integration via shared flow, also showing a satisfactory fit to the data [χ2(551, 1256) = 3455.579, p < .001, CFI = .914, RMSEA = .056]. Again, the relationship between the level of involvement and the increase of social integration was mediated by the experience of shared flow (B = .13, SE = .03, Est./S.E. = 4.18, p < .001, 99% CI [0.064, 0.280]).

In sum, our results confirmed that the relationship between the level of involvement and well-being, collective efficacy, identity fusion, and social integration were mediated by the experience of shared flow.

 

Discussion

Given that flow experiences was usually studied from the individual perspective (Jackson & Csíkszentmihályi, 2002), this research address highlights the importance of analyzing flow as a collective phenomena, which may occur during collective gatherings under socially desirable forms (Csíkszentmihályi, 1990). A number of recent studies have proposed that collective optimal experiences favor social cohesion, well-being (Páez et al., 2015; Rimé et al., 2010; Tewari, Khan, Hopkins, Srinivasan, & Reicher, 2012), and identity fusion (Whitehouse & Lanman, 2014). Still, scarce research has systematically and empirically examined the experience of flow shared by a group in collective situations. To our knowledge, this is the first study that analyzes shared flow resulting from the exposure to an intense social interaction experienced during a collective emotional gathering. In this regard, Durkheim's concept of collective effervescence and the notion of flow are convergent (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990).

The first aim of the present study was to investigate the factorial structure of the Shared Flow Scale developed on the basis of the Spanish version of the Dispositional Flow Scale (Garcia Calvo et al., 2008). CFA showed that the best and most parsimonious model was represented by a single second-order factor and nine first-order factors. The result confirmed that shared flow is a multifaceted experience, with a common variance that unifies the construct of flow, in line with previous empirical evidence concerning individual flow (Jackson & Eklund, 2002).

At the same time, the findings of the study have revealed the differential importance of the nine flow facets in this folk ritual. During the participation in tamborrada antecedents or conditions of flow experience were very high: that is, participants of tamborrada had a clear goal, perceived a balance between challenge and skill, and received a direct feedback on their performance. Participants were members of Tamborrada teams that have been training all year round, in order to be prepared for the big festival full of synchronized and coordinated activities as marching, singing, and drumming together. Accordingly, participants declared high levels of concentration the team task and a strong sense of control over the activities which were taking place during the event. As a whole, the participation was perceived as an autotelic experience, as participants enjoyed what they were doing. Nevertheless, it is important to bear in mind that not all the facets of shared flow were equally emphasized as relevant aspects of the experience of shared flow during participation in folk festival. First, the dimension of distortion of time was only moderately associated with the total shared flow. Moreover, the dimensions of merging of action and awareness and loss of self-consciousness were also less pronounced. These results contribute to clarification of the relevant importance of different domains of shared flow components in different social settings (Engeser & Schiepe-Tiska, 2012).

Secondly, this study examined positive effects of participation in folk festival and personal and collective changes through the experience of shared flow. The results confirmed indirect effects of the involvement in the activity on the increase in well-being, collective efficacy, fusion of identity with the group, and social integration via shared flow. Interestingly, these effects were stronger for collective efficacy than for well-being, fusion with the tamborrada team or social integration. In these sense, our results are in line with the findings of previous studies that have shown that the experience of shared flow experiences was associated with efficacy beliefs (Salanova et al., 2014) and increased collective efficacy beliefs (Páez et al., 2015).

Furthermore, as already detailed above, participants shared clear collective goals, perceived a balance between challenge and skill, received a direct feedback about the team behavior, and were highly concentrated on the group task and performance, with a strong sense of control over the activity. These were shown to be conditions to perceive high collective efficacy in other group tasks (Csíkszentmihályi, 1990; Jackson & Csákszentmihályi, 2002; Moradi et al., 2014; Nicholls et al., 2005). In relation to other positive effects, participation in folk festivals, through the experience of shared flow, predicts an increment in well-being. As Delle Fave & Bassi (2009) argues, individuals engage in different optimal activities that influence their individual development and improve their quality of life. At the same time, shared flow experiences imply that individuals enjoy doing a specific activity which results in positive affects and happiness (Landhauber & Keller, 2012).

On the one hand, intense participation and shared flow reinforced fusion with the tamborrada group. When experiencing a state of flow, members of a group concurrently experienced a stronger emotional bond with their coparticipants. Beyond the immediate effects on the group-team, intense participation in folk festival and the shared flow trigger social integration like perceiving social support outside the specific group of Tamborrada. In this regard, optimal experience in social contexts contributes to building a sense of community and social solidarity (McGinnis, Gentry, & Gao, 2008; Sato, 1998).

However, it is fair to acknowledge that this study presents some limitations that should be addressed in future research. The first limitation is a short period of time between longitudinal measures. In the case of our study, and considering the specificity of the evaluated ritual (tamborrada), we necessarily limited the longitudinal assessment of the outcome variables to four days after the celebration took place. Thus future research should intend to determine how long similar effects may last after the collective gathering has dissolved. Previous studies suggested that such effects are limited in time, with one week for common collective events (Páez et al., 2015; Rimé et al., 2010). The experimental mortality of longitudinal studies is another important limitation of the present investigation, although the loss of participants was low for a study of this nature.

Regarding practical implications, our study suggests that the experience of flow and collective emotions generated during an interactive social situations might be seen as even more important than individual experience of flow. When people are involved in collective participation they become intimately involved with co-present participants. Therefore, their emotional states are being constantly affected and reciprocally enhanced producing communal synergy which might then translated into enhanced feelings of belonging and cohesion, with substantial positive effects for both individuals and communities. Regarding the specific conditions that facilitate the experience of shared flow, clear goals, feedback and balance between skills and challenge could be considered as central facets that produce an autotelic experience. Therefore, we argue that in order to induce specific positive outcomes, it is important to generate conditions which could facilitate a high level of involvement and stimulate a strong shared flow. According to our findings, this synchronized and coordinated state of flow helps specifically to increase well-being and collective efficacy. Additionally, although to a lower extent, it enhances identity fusion, and social integration. In other terms, interactive social optimal experiences appear to have important impact on wellbeing and collective efficacy.

 

Conclusion

According to Csíkszentmihályi (1990), the optimal experience in social circumstances expands the flow. This study brings to light the structure and facets of shared flow as a result of the participation in a collective gathering. Distinctive aspects of the present study are the longitudinal character of the data (with measurement at three time points), and the participation of real-life folk groups or tamborradas. The results confirm that shared flow is a multifaceted experience represented by the second order factorial model with nine primary facets with differential contributions. Shared flow is specifically constructed by a clear goal, perceived balance between challenge and skill, direct feedback, high concentration/focus on the team task, a strong sense of control over the activity, and autotelic experience. In addition, the current study shows that participation in collective emotional gatherings strengthens shared emotions that foster positive effects at both individual and collective levels, especially in terms of collective efficacy.

Together, our results support Durkheim's theory (1912) and highlight that social rituals, festivals, and worship celebrations that periodically gather individuals together fulfill the function of recreating the social group and reviving shared beliefs through the experience of shared flow.

 

Acknowledgements

We wish to express our deepest appreciation to the "tamborreros/as" of St. Sebastian's Feast Day who voluntarily participated in this study and Festak-Donostia Kultura by the cooperation provided in this research project.

 

References

1. Collins, R. (2004). Interaction Ritual Chains. Princeton: Princeton University Press. doi: 10.1515/9781400851744.         [ Links ]

2. Chen, F.F. (2007) Sensitivity of Goodness of Fit Indexes to Lack of Measurement Invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14(3), 464-504. doi: 10.1080/10705510701301834.         [ Links ]

3. Csíkszentmihályi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper and Row Publishers, Inc.         [ Links ]

4. Delle Fave, A., & Bassi, M. (2009). The contribution of diversity to happiness research. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4(3), 205-207. doi: 10.1080/17439760902844319.         [ Links ]

5. Delle Fave, A. Massimini, F., & Bassi, M. (2013). Psychological selection and optimal experience across cultures. New York: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-90-481-9876-4_.         [ Links ]

6. Durkheim, E. (1912/2003). Formas elementales de la vida religiosa. Madrid: Alianza.         [ Links ]

7. Efron, B., & Tibshirani, R. J. (1993). An introduction to the bootstrap. New York: Chapman & Hall. doi: 10.1137/1036171.         [ Links ]

8. Engeser, S., & Schiepe-Tiska, A. (2012). Historical lines and an overview of current research on flow. In S. Engeser (Ed.), Advances in Flow Research Advances in flow research (pp. 1-22). New York: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-2359-1_1_.         [ Links ]

9. García Calvo, T., Jiménez, R., Santos-Rosa Ruano, F.J., Reina, R., & Cervelló, E. (2008). Psychometric Properties of the Spanish version of the Flow State Scale. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 11(2), 660-669.doi: 10.1017/S1138741600004662.         [ Links ]

10. Gómez, A., Vázquez, A., Brooks, M.L., Buhrmester, M.D., Jetten, J., & Swann, W.B. (2011). On the Nature of Identity Fusion: Insights into the Construct and a New Measure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(5), 918-933. doi: 10.1037/a0022642.         [ Links ]

11. Jackson, S., & Csíkszentmihályi, M. (2002). Fluir en el deporte. Badalona: Paidotribo.         [ Links ]

12. Jackson, S., & Eklund, R.C. (2002). Assessing flow in physical activity: The Flow State Scale-2 and Dispositional Flow Scale-2. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24, 133-150.         [ Links ]

13. Jackson, S., & Marsh, H. (1996). Development and validation of a scale to measure optimal experience: The Flow State Scale. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 18, 17-35.         [ Links ]

14. Landhäuber, A., & Keller, J. (2012). Flow and Its Affective, Cognitive, and Performance-Related Consequences. In S. Engeser (Ed.), Advances in Flow Research (pp. 65-86). New York: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-2359-1_4.         [ Links ]

15. Martínez, J. R., Guillén, F., & Feltz, D. (2011). Psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the collective efficacy questionnaire for sports. Psicothema, 23(3), 503-509. Retrieved from http://www.psicothema.com/pdf/3915.pdf.         [ Links ]

16. McGinnis, L. P., Gentry, J. W., & Gao, T. (2008). The Impact of Flow and Communitas on Enduring Involvement in Extended Service Encounters. Journal of Service Research, 11(1), 74-90. doi: 10.1177/1094670508319046.         [ Links ]

17. Mesurado, B. (2009). Actividad estructurada vs. actividad desestructurada, realizadas en solitario vs. en compañía de otros y la experiencia óptima. Anales de Psicología, 25(2), 308-315. Retrieved from http://www.revistas.um.es/analesps/article/view/87711/84501.         [ Links ]

18. Moradi, S., Nina, A.A., Rapp Ricciardi, M., Archer, T., & García, D. (2014). Exercise, character strengths, well-being, and learning climate in the prediction of performance over a 6-month period at a call center. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 497. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00497.         [ Links ]

19. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2010). Mplus User's Guide. Sixth Edition. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén.         [ Links ]

20. Nicholls, A., Polman, R., & Holt, N. (2005). The effects of individualized imagery interventions on golf performance and flow states. Athletic Insight, 7, 43-64.         [ Links ]

21. Páez, D., Rimé, B., Basabe, N., Wlodarczyk, A., & Zumeta, L. (2015). Psychosocial Effects of Perceived Emotional Synchrony in Collective Gatherings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108 (5), 711-729. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000014.         [ Links ]

22. Reddish, P., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. (2013). Let's dance together: synchrony, shared intentionality and cooperation. PloS one, 8(8), e71182. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071182.         [ Links ]

23. Richer, S., & Vallerand, R. (1998). Construction et validation de l'Échelle du sentiment d'appartenance sociale. Revue européenne depsychologie appliquée, 48, 129-137.         [ Links ]

24. Rimé, B., Páez, D., Basabe, N., & Martínez, F. (2010). Social sharing of emotion, post-traumatic growth, and emotional climate: Follow-up of Spanish citizen's response to the collective trauma of March 11th terrorist attacks in Madrid. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 1029-1045. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.700.         [ Links ]

25. Rossano, M.J. (2012). The Essential Role of Ritual in the Transmission and Reinforcement of Social Norms. Psychological Bulletin, 138(3), 529-549. doi: 10.1037/a0027038.         [ Links ]

26. Salanova, M., Rodríguez-Sánchez, A. M., Schaufeli, W. B., & Cifre, E. (2014). Flowing together: A longitudinal study of collective efficacy and collective flow among workgroups. The Journal of Psychology, 148(4), 435-455. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2013.806290.         [ Links ]

27. Sato, I. (1998). Bosozoku: flujo en las bandas japonesas motorizadas. In M. Csíkszentmihályi, & I. S. Csíkszentmihályi (Eds.), Experiencia Óptima. Estudios psicológicos del flujo en la conciencia (pp. 99-120). Bilbao: Desclée De Brouwer.         [ Links ]

28. Sawyer, R. K. (2003). Group creativity. Music, theater, collaboration. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.         [ Links ]

29. Schiepe-Tiska, A., & Engeser, S. (2012). Flow in Nonachievement Situations. In S. Engeser (Ed.). Advances in flow research (pp. 87-107). New York: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-2359-1_5.         [ Links ]

30. Steiger, J.H. (2007). Understanding the limitations of global fit assessment in structural equation modeling. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(5), 893-98. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.017.         [ Links ]

31. Tewari S., Khan S., Hopkins, N., Srinivasan, N., & Reicher, S. (2012). Participation in mass gatherings can benefit well-being: Longitudinal and control data from a North Indian Hindu pilgrimage event. PloS One, 7(10), e47291. doi: 10.1371 / journal.pone.0047291.         [ Links ]

32. Vázquez, C., & Hervás, G. (2012). Addressing current challenges in crosscultural measurement of well-being: The Pemberton Happiness Index. In A. Delle Fave, & H.H. Koop (Eds.), Anthology on Cross Cultural Advancements in Positive Psychology (pp. 31-49). New York: Springer-Verlag. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-4611-4_3.         [ Links ]

33. von Scheve, C., & Ismer, S. (2013). Towards a theory of collective emotions. Emotion Review, 5, 406-413. doi: 10.1177/1754073913484170.         [ Links ]

34. von Scheve, C., & Salmela, M. (Eds.) (2014). Collective Emotions. New York: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199659180.001.0001.         [ Links ]

35. Walker, C. K. (2010). Experiencing flow: Is doing it together better than doing it alone? The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5, 3-11. doi: 10.1080/17439760903271116.         [ Links ]

36. Whitehouse, H., & Lanman, J.A. (2014). The Ties That Bind Us: Ritual, Fusion, and Identification. Journal of Current Anthropology, 55(6), 674-695. doi:10.1086/678698.         [ Links ]

37. Zumeta, L., Basabe, N., Telletxea, S., Amutio, A., & Bobowik, M. (in press). Shared flow in collective sport physical activity and subjective wellbeing. EJIHPE, European Journal of Investigation in Health Psychology and Education.         [ Links ]

38. Zumeta, L., Oriol, X., Telletxea, S., Amutio, A., & Basabe, N. (2016). Collective Efficacy in Sports and Physical Activities: Perceived Emotional Synchrony and Shared Flow. Frontiers in Psychology. Movement Science and Sport Psychology. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01960.         [ Links ]

39. Zumeta. L., Wlodarczyk, A., Basabe, N., Telletxea, S., Alves, D., & Puente, A. (2013). Shared flow and emotional communion in collective activities. Poster on 12th ECPA. July, Donostia/San Sebastián, Spain. doi: 10.1080/13697130512331390481.         [ Links ]

 

 

Correspondence:
Larraitz Zumeta.
Departamento de Psicología Social y
Metodología de las Ciencias del Comportamiento.
Facultad de Psicología.
Universidad del País Vasco.
Avenida Tolosa 70,
20018 San Sebastián (Spain).
E-mail: larraitznerea.zumeta@ehu.eus

Article received: 02-07-2015
Revised: 11-05-2016
Accepted: 06-06-2016

Creative Commons License Todo el contenido de esta revista, excepto dónde está identificado, está bajo una Licencia Creative Commons