SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.33 número3Propiedades psicométricas de la Liverpool Stoicism Scale (LSS) en una cohorte de pacientes con cáncer resecado en tratamiento adyuvanteCómo implementar el modelo centrado en la familia en la intervención temprana índice de autoresíndice de assuntospesquisa de artigos
Home Pagelista alfabética de periódicos  

Serviços Personalizados

Journal

Artigo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • Em processo de indexaçãoCitado por Google
  • Não possue artigos similaresSimilares em SciELO
  • Em processo de indexaçãoSimilares em Google

Compartilhar


Anales de Psicología

versão On-line ISSN 1695-2294versão impressa ISSN 0212-9728

Anal. Psicol. vol.33 no.3 Murcia Out. 2017

https://dx.doi.org/10.6018/analesps.33.3.248351 

 

 

Facing death. Student's thoughts towards the feeling of their own death

La muerte ante uno mismo. Respuestas de jóvenes estudiantes ante el pensamiento de la propia muerte

 

 

Celia Martí-García1,2, Manuel Fernández-Alcántara1,3, Laura Ruiz-Martín4, Rafael Montoya-Juárez1,5, Cesar Hueso-Montoro1,5 and M. Paz García-Caro1,5

1 Centro de Investigación Mente, Cerebro y Comportamiento (CIMCyC). (Spain).
2 Universidad de Málaga, Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud. (Spain).
3 Universidad de Granada, Facultad de Psicología. (Spain).
4 Hospital Son Llàtzer. Palma de Mallorca. (Spain).
5 Universidad de Granada, Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud. (Spain).

Correspondence

 

 


ABSTRACT

Narratives produced by thinking and reflecting about death and dying may be themselves a source of new knowledge in the study of anxiety and attitudes toward death. A qualitative descriptive study aimed to explore the features of thoughts, emotions and feelings about young people's own death, (nursing students) was conducted. 85 students voluntarily answered a self-administered questionnaire with open questions based on Mortality Salience, used in the Terror Management Theory. Content analysis using Atlas.ti software was performed. When reflecting upon their own death, participants reported substantive emotions such as fear, pain, distress, sadness or loneliness. The fear of their own death was specified as the inability to achieve goals in life, to leave the family and the dying process itself. When reflecting upon been physically dead, attributes of the decomposition of the body, spiritual attributes, beliefs, denial and preferences were identified. Cultural worldview, values and beliefs of each person were remarkably present when thinking about their own death.

Key words: Death Attitudes; Terror Management Theory; Mortality Salience; Emotion; Nursing; Qualitative Study.


RESUMEN

Las narraciones sobre lo que evoca pensar en la propia muerte y describir los pensamientos y sentimientos sobre el morir, pueden ser por sí mismas una fuente de conocimiento nuevo en el estudio de la ansiedad y las actitudes ante la muerte. Se llevó a cabo un estudio cualitativo descriptivo con el objetivo de explorar las características de los pensamientos, las emociones y sentimientos sobre la propia muerte de jóvenes, estudiantes de enfermería. 85 estudiantes respondieron voluntariamente un cuestionario autoadministrado de preguntas abiertas basadas en la Mortality Salience empleada en la Teoría de la Gestión del Terror. El análisis se realizó mediante la estrategia de análisis de contenido con el programa Atlas ti. Ante el pensamiento sobre la propia muerte respondieron con los sustantivos miedo, dolor, angustia, tristeza o soledad. El miedo a la propia muerte se concretó como la imposibilidad de lograr las metas en la vida, dejar a la familia y el proceso de muerte en sí mismo. Ante las creencias sobre estar físicamente muerto señalaron atributos de la descomposición del cuerpo, atributos espirituales, creencias, negación y preferencias. La visión cultural, los valores y creencias de cada persona, se encuentran muy presentes cuando piensan en su propia muerte.

Palabras clave: Actitud ante la muerte; Teoría de la Gestión del Terror; Emoción; Enfermería; Estudio cualitativo.


 

Introduction

Death is a natural phenomenon, inherent to the human condition, and the inevitable consequence of life. Nevertheless, death or its proximity generates a series of emotional responses, including frustration, fear, anxiety and depression (Cruz-Quintana, 2007).

Society has adapted poorly to death, evolving from attitudes of acceptance and coping towards an aura of taboo regarding the subject (Edo-Gual, Tomás-Sábado & Aradilla-Herrero, 2011). In the framework of the Western model of medical attention, death is considered a failure and so there is a social tendency to hide and medicalise it (Colell, Limonero & Otero, 2003; Chocarro-González, Gónzalez-Fernández, Salvadores-Fuentes & Venturi-Medina, 2012). This hinders the integration of death as a natural process and distances the individual from awareness and reflection on his/her own mortality and that of others.

A major area of research interest is that of emotions and attitudes towards death. In general, this event generates a wide range of attitudes and emotions, chief among which are fear and/or anxiety (Fortner & Neimeyer, 1999; Neimeyer, 1994, 2005; Templer et al., 2006). To date, most studies in this area have focused on measuring anxiety and its relation to other factors, such as age or religion (Lester, Templer & Abdel-Khalek, 2007; Neimeyer, Currier, Coleman, Tomer & Samuel, 2011), applying methods focused mainly on quantitative analysis of the data. However, this focus shifted with the development of terror management theory (TMT), which emphasises the effects on daily life of attitudes towards death.

According to Ernest Becker (2003), humans, like other living beings, have a strong instinct to survive and prolong life. However, unlike other organisms, humans are aware of the inevitability of death. TMT experiments by Pyszczynski, Greenberg, Solomon & Maxfield (2006) showed that consciousness of the finitude of life evokes anxiety and terror and, under certain circumstances, triggers a series of avoidance mechanisms. On the one hand, proximal defences are activated when thoughts of death come to the forefront of consciousness; such defences are based on distraction or trivialisation, aimed at driving away such thoughts from the mind. Distal defences, on the other hand, prevent the thoughts of death that are still accessible from remaining in the subconscious (Pyszczynski, Solomon & Greenberg, 2015). Two main distal defences have been described: self-esteem and belief in the validity of one's own cultural outlook on the world and its associated values (Burgin, Sanders, Vandellen & Martin, 2012; Burke, Martens & Faucher, 2010; Routledge, Juhl & Vess, 2013). Unconsciously, both of these mechanisms help people overcome the terror associated with their own death, by providing a sense of order and replacing the reality of existential death with a possible later life, whether literal or symbolic (Hohman & Hogg, 2011).

In exploring this question, most studies of TMT have adopted the hypothesis of mortality salience (MS) (Burke et al., 2010), by which thoughts are spurred about one's own mortality. This hypothesis holds that if distal defences are erected to barricade the entrance to consciousness against thoughts concerning death, then increasing accessibility to such thoughts would strengthen the compulsion to maintain the psychological structures underpinning these defences, thus making them more necessary. In many studies of MS, members of the experimental group are asked to write about their own death, while those in the control group are instructed to write about an event unrelated to death (although, in many cases, one with negative connotations). The results obtained from such studies show that much of the behaviour we present and experience on a daily basis is influenced by unconscious thoughts about our own death (Burke et al., 2010; Burgin et al., 2012), reinforcing the defence of ideals or values related to distal defences.

The most common way in which MS is addressed and analysed (and which has been used in 79.8% of such studies) (Burgin et al., 2012) is by means of the Mortality Attitudes Personality Survey (Rosenblatt, Greenberg, Solomon, Pyszczynski & Lyon, 1989), which contains two direct questions on the thoughts aroused by death and about what the respondents believe will happen when they die and when they are dead.

The first of these, the Emotion Question ("Briefly describe the emotions that the thought of your own death arouses in you"), requires participants to express their thoughts, feelings and experiences. The second, the After-life Question ("Jot down as specifically as you can what you think will happen to you as you physically die, and once you are physically dead"), reports on participants' cultural values and beliefs (Burgin et al., 2012). In most studies, these questions are not analysed in depth and no conclusions are drawn from the participants' narratives (Kastenbaum & Heflick, 2011), since what is sought is to determine the effect produced, in making these responses, on another activity that is not directly related to the question of mortality.

An exception to this pattern was the study by Kastenbaum and Heflick (2011), in which the narratives offered by university students who took part in TMT-associated experiments were used to identify various outcomes, which included feelings of anxiety and sadness. These first-person narratives on the feelings evoked in thinking about one's own death and in describing the thoughts and feelings about its consequences, i.e., death itself and its product (the corpse), may constitute a source of new knowledge regarding anxiety and attitudes towards death. However, as the authors acknowledge, their study is subject to certain major limitations. First, the questions were asked as part of a larger study that was not intended to explore the answers to MS questions but to activate accessibility to thoughts about death, in order to evaluate their influence on other types of behaviour. Accordingly, other variables may have influenced the responses made. Second, the method of analysis employed, grouping into clusters the different adjectives used by the participants, impedes an in-depth exploration of subjective outlooks on death. For example, one such cluster contained 27 different adjectives, including calm, curious, excited and happy, which is indicative of the great heterogeneity in this area (Kastenbaum and Heflick, 2011).

In health sciences, diverse studies have referred to the need for medical personnel to analyse and be aware of their own ideas about death and dying, as part of the preparation needed in order to address the process of mortality in their relations with patients and their families, to reduce anxiety and to enhance people's ability to cope with the situation (Chow, 2013; Mason & Ellershaw, 2010; Schillerstrom, Sánchez-Reilly & O'Donnell, 2012; Schmidt-RioValle et al., 2012). Studies of emotional intelligence have emphasised how important it is for nurses to know how to effectively manage the emotions aroused by continued contact with illness and death, and the need for specific emotional preparation to neutralise avoidance conduct and to minimise inadequate responses that might impede professional development and provoke emotional conflicts (Pineda-Galán, 2012; Sánchez-Rueda, 2014).

The question arises, then, of whether the MS survey (aimed at a specific population sample) might provide a means of identifying responses to overriding thoughts about one's own death, and whether possession of this knowledge would improve attitudes and responses to death and dying.

To evaluate the potential and applicability of the MS questionnaire, this study takes into account two fundamental requirements: 1) the study environment must be relevant and appropriate to the area of research; 2) the results obtained must enable specific improvements to be applied. The field of education was considered the most appropriate and feasible for these two conditions to be met. In this respect, moreover, healthcare staff constitute an especially vulnerable population group because of their direct involvement in end-of-life processes. After considering various options, it was concluded that the nursing degree course offered at the University of Granada provided the necessary applicability, as it incorporated a specific course subject in this area, namely palliative care, in the second year. Therefore, it would be possible to measure the effect of specific occupational training on students' thoughts regarding these questions.

The study presented in this paper explores the characteristics of thoughts, emotions and feelings about death, experienced after specific training in this field, expressed by nursing students in response to an open-ended questionnaire focused on mortality salience (MS). This questionnaire seeks to provide detailed information about the baseline state of mind among young people about their own death. We also consider the influence of personal characteristics such as age and sex, and of any personal and/or professional experience of mortality, such as having lived with relatives or cared for patients faced with serious illness and/or death.

The main aim of this study, therefore, is to describe the characteristics of the responses evoked by thinking about one's own death and the situations of dying and of physical death, according to the opinions expressed by a group of nursing students. A secondary objective was to determine the effect of certain factors, such as age, sex, and personal and/or professional experience, on the responses made.

 

Method

Design

This qualitative observational descriptive study was carried out using an open-ended questionnaire, and syntactic and semantic content analysis (Ato, López & Benavente, 2013).

Participants

The study participants were selected by intentional sampling. Among the 130 students enrolled in each course year, 88 who had completed the first year of the degree course in nursing studies at the Health Sciences Faculty of the University of Granada during the academic years 2011-2012 and 2012-2013 volunteered to take part. Three responses were excluded because the students' questionnaire data were incomplete, and so the final sample was composed of 85 students (Table 1).

 

 

Instruments

The survey was self-administered and contained the following items:

- Sociodemographic data: age, sex.

- Previous personal experience in caring for relatives with serious or terminal illness (YES/NO).

- Previous professional experience in caring for patients with serious or terminal illness (YES/NO).

- Open questions: 1) Briefly describe the emotions aroused by the thought of your own death; 2) Write down as specifically as you can what you think will happen to you physically when you are dying and once you are physically dead.

The open questions were derived from the Mortality Attitudes Personality Survey (Rosenblatt et al., 1989), used in MS studies in Terror Management Theory.

Procedure

To evaluate the potential and applicability of the MS questionnaire, this study takes into account two fundamental requirements: 1) the study environment must be relevant and appropriate to the area of research; 2) the results obtained must enable specific improvements to be applied. The field of education was considered the most appropriate and feasible for these two conditions to be met. In this respect, moreover, healthcare staff constitute an especially vulnerable population group because of their direct involvement in end-of-life processes. After considering various options, it was concluded that the nursing degree course offered at the University of Granada provided the necessary applicability, as it incorporated a specific course subject in this area, namely palliative care, in the second year. Therefore, it would be possible to measure the effect of specific occupational training on students' thoughts regarding these questions.

Therefore, on the first day of the second-year degree course, simultaneously in each of the classrooms in use by these students, the research project was explained to those present and verbal consent to voluntary participation was requested. A psychologist was present during the data compilation process to offer advice and support if any questions caused discomfort. Thus, doubts were resolved and clarifications provided. The students filled in the questionnaire immediately, in the classroom, anonymously and voluntarily, and were advised that if they did not wish to participate or if they did not feel able to continue, they could hand in the questionnaire, leave the classroom and request the intervention of the psychologist if this was felt necessary. No participant requested this type of assistance. The time taken to complete the questionnaire ranged from nine to twelve minutes. The confidentiality of the data was ensured by coding the identification of the questionnaires. At no time were the students asked for their name or place of origin.

Data analysis

The data obtained were analysed using ATLAS.ti 6.2 software. To do so, the first steps were to create the Hermeneutic Unit and to generate the primary documents containing the literal answers given to each questionnaire.

For the content analysis, we followed the Mayring (2000) model, based first on an inductive and then on a deductive sequence. In this process, the meaning of each sentence was considered, in search of units of meaning, themes and subthemes.

After an initial reading of the texts, a series of main categories and sub-categories related to the questionnaire topics were extracted. These were then contrasted and reformulated during the analysis of the information (Table 2). Subsequently, the textual expressions in the primary documents were classified and coded. A syntactic analysis of the data was then performed, by word count, which supplied the frequency of appearance of each code. In addition, a semantic analysis was conducted to determine the meaning of the words employed and to analyse the themes and categories proposed (Amezcua & Galvez-Toro, 2002). Finally, the relations between the different codes were identified, and networks and tables created for the preparation of the final report and the extraction of results.

 

 

Strategies were included to improve the reliability of the data, regarding both the process and the results obtained, in accordance with the criteria established by Lincoln and Guba (1985) regarding reliability, credibility and auditability. Two external experts in qualitative analysis, acting as triangulation researchers, then reviewed the code generation, the results obtained and the data analysis performed (Denzin, 1989; Flick, 1992).

 

Results

Emotions aroused by thoughts about one's own death

In response to the survey item, "Briefly describe the emotions aroused by the thought of your own death", two main categories were identified: i) emotions aroused by the thought of one's own death; ii) specific thoughts aroused on contemplating one's own death.

Emotions

Among the most frequently described emotions were fear (20.2%), pain (15.6%), anguish (13.2%), sadness (10.7%), anxiety (10.1%), loneliness (6.8%) and uncertainty (6.5%). Fear was the emotion most often mentioned in all the conditions analysed except when the participant had previous professional experience in the care of dying patients and among those over 26 years of age (Table 3).

There were no significant differences between the percentages of men and women who cited fear and pain as the emotions most frequently aroused. However, women referred frequently to sadness and anguish, while men placed greater emphasis on anxiety and loneliness. By age groups, the students aged under 20 years most often cited anxiety and sadness, while those aged 20 to 25 years did so regarding pain and sadness, and in those aged 26 years or older, the main emotion reflected was loneliness.

When personal experience in caring for a dying relative was taken into consideration, the participants cited the emotions of pain and fear in the same proportions as above, but made less reference to sadness. However, if the experience in question involved a family member with a severe illness, the percentage of students mentioning fear and anguish increased. Among those with previous professional experience of caring for a dying patient, fear was not mentioned at all, but when the experience concerned caring for a patient with a severe illness, the emotions of fear, pain and anguish were cited in the same percentages as before.

Thoughts

When the students were asked about the emotions aroused by the thought of their own death, many also referred to their thoughts or feelings in this respect. Five main areas (codes) were identified: i) family; ii) uncertainty about death; iii) achieving one's goals; iv) finitude; v) acceptance of death.

-The family

Most of the students' observations concerned feelings of sadness at leaving their family or at not being able to see them anymore:

"The idea of no longer being with my loved ones makes me unhappy".

They also expressed concern about how they would cope with death or about the suffering of their loved ones during this time:

"I fear the suffering of my family and close friends. If I knew they wouldn't suffer, the thought of dying wouldn't be so bad".

-Uncertainty about death

One topic that frequently arose was the fear caused by uncertainty about what would come after death, related to the doubts present in one's beliefs:

"Fear of not knowing what there is after death".

"Thinking about my own death distresses me because doubts keep going round and round my head: what will it be like, what will be there ... Or is there anything at all after death?".

Moreover, in many cases there is a sense of anguish provoked by ignorance of the experience of death, by not knowing what will happen, or when and how we will die. It was at this point that many of the respondents recognised they were afraid of the process of death, not only regarding physical or emotional suffering, but also regarding the conditions in which death would take place:

"The only thing that worries me is the pain that I may suffer, or immobility, or being in a coma, before my death".

"I'm not afraid of death, but of dying; I don't want to suffer or die alone. I don't mind dying, if my loved ones are with me at the time".

"Not knowing when I'm going to die is disquieting".

-Leaving everything done, achieving life's ambitions

Some of the respondents' views referred to the importance of achieving lifetime goals and of living all the experiences they wished to enjoy before death. In other words, these respondents hoped for a death that left no loose ends:

"My death worries me when I think about everything I'd like to do before I die".

"My death, or thinking about it, makes me wonder: when it happens, will I have achieved all my goals?".

However, among a small group of participants, thoughts about their own death led them to reflect on the importance of making the most of life and of enjoying the time available.

"The thought of my death makes me happy for the time I've had to live".

"I enjoy life and I'm not obsessed about death. You have to take advantage of what life has to offer".

-Finitude

The sense of finitude was also present in the students' words; the idea of ceasing to think, of no longer existing, terrified them:

"It frightens me, especially the fact of ceasing to exist and no longer thinking".

"It frightens me, the idea of not feeling, not seeing, not hearing, not being able to share and interact with my friends and family, I'm afraid of not being".

-Acceptance of death

Despite all the fears aroused by thinking about death, many students referred to the acceptance of one's own death as a natural act, although many linked this acceptance to the passage of time, since they assumed death would take place in the distant future:

"In the long term, I accept it perfectly well, it's only natural".

"My own death doesn't worry me, it's something I've always assumed will happen".

Beliefs about what will happen physically when dying and once dead

In reference to the question, "Write down as specifically as you can what you believe will happen to you physically when you are dying and once you are physically dead", a distinction should be made between the two situations posed (dying and being dead) in our analysis of the information provided.

Beliefs about physically dying

- Description of the physical process of dying

The students' descriptions of what may be experienced in physically dying revealed a wide variety of perceptions regarding the physical signs and symptoms that may occur. The physical states most commonly referred to were the failure of vital organs (14.17%), followed closely by weakness (12.9%), pain (11.81%), cardiorespiratory arrest (8.66%) and loss of consciousness (6.69%). Some students described the process using the metaphor of "switching off'.

"Little by little, I willprogressively be switched off."

"I won't need to breathe, or think; little by little, like a machine that's running out of battery power, I'll run down and switch off, when everything goes dark".

- Feelings about the moment of death

In stating what they believed would physically happen to them when they were dying, some participants also referred to the feelings or emotions they would experience during their death. Terms used for this included distress (18.75%), anxiety (12.5%), sadness (10.42%) and awareness of death itself (10.42%), expressed in forms such as the following:

"When you are dying, I think that little by little you come to accept the idea that you are going to die".

Analysis of these results, according to the sociodemographic data compiled, showed the differences in the percentages to be minimal and non-significant.

Beliefs about beingphysically dead

Two types of classification code were obtained: (i) corpse-related qualities: body decomposition and physical and spiritual attributes (see Figure 1); (ii) personal qualities and attitudes: beliefs, denial and preferences (see Table 4).

- Attributes of body decomposition, and physical and spiritual attributes.

Many references were made to spiritual attributes, especially by the male students. By age groups, more references to spiritual attributes were made by those aged less than 26 years.

Previous personal experience was associated with the number of references made to spiritual attributes, regarding dying and severely ill relatives. Nearly all of the students with previous professional experience referred to the attributes of body decomposition, in descriptions of caring for dying patients, and also referred to spiritual attributes in the case of severely ill persons (Table 4).

Regarding physical change, the students mentioned the decomposition, putrefaction and disintegration of the corpse after death, the physical attributes of a corpse, such as coldness, pallor, rigidity (or rigor mortis) and immobility. With respect to spiritual attributes, they used existential expressions such as feeling nothing, darkness or simply "nothing" (Figure 1).

- Personal qualities and attitudes: beliefs, denial and preferences

Half of the students referred to doubts regarding life after death:

"l don't know, either, if there is anything after death, but I hope so".

On the other hand, 22.2% claimed to have no belief in an afterlife:

"l view it simply as an end; l don't believe in a life after death".

The other remarks made were manifestations of personal beliefs, unrelated to religion:

"Dust to dust, ashes to ashes; that's it".

"All emotions, thoughts and feelings will disappear, it's farewell to the soul".

71.43% of the comments made (n = 35) regarding the denial of death referred to avoiding thinking about it:

"I get really anxious and avoid thinking about it, because when I do I have problems in sleeping and can't concentrate".

However, there were also expressions of indifference or doubt:

"I don't worry too much about how I will die".

"I honestly can't imagine the moment when I'll be dying or when I'll be dead".

Another significant aspect of the comments made concerned the students' preferences (n = 70). Thus, 35.7% would prefer to be cremated, versus 7.14% who wished to be buried. 8.57% would donate their organs, while 7.14% hoped to die in the company of their loved ones.

The treatment granted the body once dead (i.e. cremation or burial) was more often referred to by the male students, while the women more frequently commented on desirable conditions during the process of dying, such as the absence of pain and suffering, and that the process should be rapid and/or in the company of one's family.

In this area, the wish that was most commonly expressed was to be cremated after death. This was especially so among those with experience in caring for the dying, together with the wish to die accompanied by loved ones and to be able to say goodbye to them before dying.

 

Discussion

The aim of this study is to obtain a detailed description of the main considerations associated with the thought of one's own death and of situations of dying, among a student population. This experiment, in which intrusive thinking about one's own death was provoked among a group of young people, has enhanced our understanding of the mental representation of death and dying, expressed in the first person. The views given can be considered representative of the baseline state in this respect, at the experiential level. Our analysis of the narratives evoked by thoughts about one's own death shows that the fear of dying, the pain of thinking about one's own death, the anguish and sadness aroused and the suffering related to what is lost or left behind are the main features of the emotions associated with one's own death. The beliefs associated with the two states into which we inquired, namely physically dying and being physically dead, are intermixed, including both purely physical conditions and emotions and feelings. In their descriptions of what they believe will happen to them physically, our respondents cannot ignore what they feel or believe they will feel.

The results obtained concerning the emotions evoked in thinking about one's own death coincide with the findings of Kastenbaum and Heflick (2011), who reported that anxiety and sadness were the principal emotions aroused. In the present study, together with these two emotions, other, related feelings were observed; thus, in the respondents' discourse, terms such as anguish, fear, pain and uncertainty were commonly employed to describe the emotional state evoked.

In both cases, the female respondents in our study more frequently used terms related to distress, sadness and uncertainty (for example, anxiety, preoccupation and a sense of perplexity). Among the men, on the other hand, the responses made were more often expressed in terms of anxiety and loneliness. In the main, they did not employ terms disassociating themselves from death. In this respect, Kastenbaum and Heflick (2011) observed respondents' use of terms that detached them from the idea, in apathetic responses such as "I don't cart".

The differences in the expressions used by our male and female respondents might be interpreted as gender-based nuances. However, in our study, the terms that were most frequently used by both men and women were, first, fear and second, pain. For Kastenbaum and Heflick (2011), the main emotions described by women were first anxiety and then sadness; for men, this order was reversed. Other studies, using quantitative measures, have also found that women feel greater anxiety about death than men (Tomas-Sabado & Gomez-Benito, 2003). These differences are probably related to a methodological diversity among the studies. In consequence, the results obtained concerning the open expression of emotions are not comparable with those reported by studies in which specific questionnaires are used (Tomas-Sabado & Gomez-Benito, 2003). In this respect, when they are expressed in terms of personal experience and emotions, the answers given may depend more on the respondents' own outlook on the world and on their personal values (Chocarro-Gonzalez, 2010) than on their gender.

In order to interpret the influence of the respondents' age on the responses made, it should be taken into account that 87% of the respondents were under 26 years of age. The answers given may be considered logical, in that younger persons tend to be more concerned with insecurity regarding death, given its lack of proximity. This is in accordance with the cultural values of our society, where death is considered a natural outcome or process, something that affects the elderly (Chocarro-Gonzalez et al., 2012). Our analysis revealed, moreover, that the younger students made a greater number of references to fear and anxiety, thus confirming previous findings (Edo-Gual et al., 2011; Limonero, Tomas-Sabado, Fernandez-Castro, Cladellas & Gomez -Benito, 2010; Lyke, 2013). Thus, with age there is a greater experience of loss and unpleasant circumstances, which spurs the development of more effective coping strategies to reduce feelings of anxiety and fear.

Many of the students also expressed concerns regarding the moment of their own death. In this respect, the ideas most commonly cited included the fear of not being able to achieve one's goals (Kastenbaum & Heflick, 2011; Liu et al., 2011) and the importance of the family. This latter concern was to the forefront of many of the narratives offered, both because of the sadness at ceasing to be with their family members (Liu et al., 2011; Shih, Gau, Lin, Pong & Lin et al., 2006) and from concern as to how the family would cope with their death (Kastenbaum & Heflick, 2011). In other words, the respondents were preoccupied by the grief that their own death would cause to others. Interestingly, these concerns bear a great similarity to those expressed by patients in an advanced stage of terminal illness. Montoya-Juarez et al. (2013) highlighted various emotional responses by such patients, in a study population derived from the same cultural environment as that of the participants in the present study. These patients expressed concern about the "loose ends" they would leave behind, mainly related to the future of their families. Similar concerns have been observed in other studies (Baile, Palmer, Bruera & Parker, 2010; Ryan, 2005) and in the handbook Nearing the End of Life published by the American Cancer Society (2014). The similarity of some of these thoughts regarding one's own death, in different cultural contexts, may be indicative of a conception of death and dying that is shared and generalised within developed societies, as has been noted by authors such as Louis-Vicent Thomas (1983) and Philippe Ariès (2005).

Another viewpoint is that what is generalised may be the emotional origin. This was the outlook expressed by Izard (2009), who developed the concept of "emotion schemas". This term refers to emotion in dynamic interaction with perceptual and cognitive processes that influence the mind and behaviour. These processes would be elicited by activation but also by images, memories and thoughts, and would be influenced by individual differences, learning and the cultural and social context (Zerpa, 2009). Such schemas could explain how a specific emotional state comes to be inscribed in the brain, from the relation between a basic emotion and self-awareness (Damasio, 2005), which is expressed in accordance with the sociocultural context. Previous studies have reported the existence of a specific emotional schema for images of death (Martí-García et al., 2014). In the present case, the eliciting power of MS questions is apparent, in a particular context in which the education received would modulate perceptions. Our study adopts a new approach and employs a novel method by which such differences might be controlled in future research.

Another recurrent theme in the narratives was the generalised uncertainty about what will happen after death. This is one of the main effects highlighted in MS studies, since reminders of mortality, as well as evoking our fear of death, aggravate existential uncertainty about what might come next (Van des Bos, 2009).

Corroborating previous studies (Chocarro-Gonzalez et al., 2012), our findings revealed indications of the denial of death, in the sense of avoiding thinking about it or being unable to imagine any aspect related to it. In the narratives of those participants capable of describing their death, a frequent occurrence is the expression of their desires at the moment of death and once they are dead, taking advantage of the opportunity offered by this study to state their preferences. These included the desire to die at home, to donate their organs, not to extend their life by unnecessary procedures and to be cremated after death. Many such preferences have been recorded in previous studies on end-of-life decisions among the Spanish population (Wanden-Berghe, Guardiola- Wanden-Berghe & Sanz-Valero, 2009).

Finally, although prior experience in coping with end-of-life processes was only evaluated on the basis of the respondents' experiences in caring for patients or relatives with severe and/or terminal illnesses, there was a notable increase in references to the most prevalent emotions, such as anxiety or pain, among those who had cared for family members. However, this was not the case with respect to fear, which was absent from the narratives of respondents with professional experience in caring for a dying patient, or sadness, which in general had no significantly increased presence in the narratives.

The results obtained suggest that emotions, thoughts and beliefs are appropriate areas on which to focus educational programmes in the health sciences regarding personal views on mortality, both one's own death and that of others, thus enhancing understanding in this regard. Studies have shown that training in communication skills and palliative care can significantly reduce levels of anxiety about death (Schmidt-Rio Valle et al., 2012) or modify reactions to emotional stimuli directly related to death (Martí-García et al., 2014).

The results obtained suggest that the management of emotions, by means of strategies related to emotional intelligence, should be incorporated into training programmes for students and healthcare professionals, as a means of recognising one's own feelings and those of others, in order to be motivated and to properly manage relationships with others and with oneself. In the field of health care, the management of empathy, i.e., the ability to recognise the emotional states of others and on that basis react in a socially appropriate way, is of fundamental importance (Sánchez Rueda, 2014; Pineda Galán, 2012).

This study presents the following strengths and limitations. On the positive side, it is one of the few studies conducted in this area in which an in-depth qualitative analysis is made of health sciences students' emotions regarding the subject of death, using a research method based on the MS paradigm. Moreover, this analysis highlights a wealth of expressions and emotions that were not revealed in previous studies. On the other hand, our data analysis suffers an important limitation in that we are unaware whether the respondents, in their narratives, are saying what they believe they will feel when the situation of mortality arises or, on the contrary, whether this is what they are feeling at the present moment, on considering the prospect. This ambiguity must be taken into account in any generalisation of the results obtained. Furthermore, this study does not obtain detailed characteristics of the personal and/or professional experience of the respondents except that it refers to the death or serious illness of family members or patients. No other information was requested, to avoid recalling unwelcome memories and to maintain the necessary conditions of neutrality. However, it was important to determine the respondents' previous experience in this respect, as the sampling procedure was intentional, based on voluntary participation. Nevertheless, this fact may have introduced bias into the results obtained.

In conclusion, the MS questionnaire examined in this study was found to be a useful means of investigating responses to intrusive thinking about one's own death, among a student population. Among the main emotions aroused in this respect are fear of dying, pain in thinking about one's own death, anguish, sadness and suffering related to what is lost or left behind. The variables of sex, age, and prior experience partly determined the variability in the responses evoked. However, the more profound responses, such as beliefs, seem to depend more on each individual's worldview and personal values.

 

References

1. American Cáncer Society. Cuando el final de la vida se acerca (en línea) Copyright American Cancer Society, 2014. (Fecha de consulta: 21 Diciembre 2015) Disponible en: http://www.cancer.org/acs/groups/cid /documents/webcontent/002900-pdf.pdf.         [ Links ]

2. Amezcua, M., y Gálvez Toro, A. (2002). Los modos de análisis en investigación cualitativa en salud: perspectiva crítica y reflexiones en voz alta. Revista Española de Salud Pública, 76(5), 423-36.         [ Links ]

3. Ariès, P (2005) Historia de la muerte en occidente: desde la edad media hasta nuestros dias. 3a ed. Barcelona: El acantilado.         [ Links ]

4. Ato, M., López, J. J., y Benavente, A. (2013). Un sistema de clasificación de los diseños de investigación en psicología. Anales de psicología, 29(3), 1038-1059. http://dx.doi.org/10.6018/analesps.29.3T78511.         [ Links ]

5. Baile, W. F., Palmer, J. L., Bruera, E., and Parker, P. A. (2011). Assessment of palliative care cancer patients' most important concerns. Supportive Care in Cancer, 19(4), 475-481. doi: 10.1007/s00520-010-0839-4.         [ Links ]

6. Becker, E. (2003). La negación de la muerte. Barcelona: Kairós.         [ Links ]

7. Burgin, C. J., Sanders, M. A., Vandellen, M. R., and Martin, L. L. (2012). Breaking apart the typical mortality salience manipulation: Two questions, two outcomes. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42(4), 521-532. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.1845.         [ Links ]

8. Burke, B. L., Martens, A., and Faucher, E. H. (2010). Two decades of terror management theory: A meta-analysis of mortality salience research. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14(2), 155-195. doi: 10.1177/1088868309352321.         [ Links ]

9. Chocarro-González, L. (2010). Representación social de la muerte entre los profesionales sanitarios: una aproximación psicosociológica desde el análisis del discurso. (Tesis Doctoral). Facultad de Ciencias Políticas y Sociología Departamento de Psicología Social. Universidad Complutense de Madrid. Madrid. Disponible en: http://eprints.ucm.es/11998/1/T32597.pdf.         [ Links ]

10. Chocarro-González, L., Gónzalez-Fernández, R., Salvadores-Fuentes, P., y Venturi-Medina, C. (2012). Negación de la muerte y su repercusión en los cuidados. Medicina paliativa, 19(7), 148-54.         [ Links ]

11. Chow, A. Y. M. (2013). Developing emotional competence of social workers of end-of-life and bereavement care. British Journal of Social Work, 43(2), 373-393. doi: 10.1093/bjsw/bct030.         [ Links ]

12. Cruz-Quintana F. (2007). Miedo a la muerte. En Álava MJ. (Coord) Enciclopedia de la Psicología. La Psicología que nos Ayuda a Vivir (913-938). Madrid: La Esfera de los Libros.         [ Links ]

13. Damasio, A. (2005). En busca de Spinoga. Neurobiología de la emoción y los sentimientos. Barcelona: Crítica.         [ Links ]

14. Denzin, N. K. (1989). The Research Act. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall        [ Links ]

15. Dunne, S., Gallagher, P., and Matthews, A. (2015). Existential threat or dissociative response? Examining defensive avoidance of point-of-care testing devices through a terror management theory framework. Death studies, 39(1), 30-38. DOI:10.1080/07481187.2014.885469.         [ Links ]

16. Flick, U. (1992). Triangulation Revisited: Strategy of or alternative to validation of qualitative data. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 22, 175-197. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5914.1992.tb00215.x.         [ Links ]

17. Fortner, B. V., and Neimeyer, R. A. (1999). Death anxiety in older adults: A quantitative review. Death Studies, 23(5), 387-411. doi:10.1080/074811899200920.         [ Links ]

18. Gual, M., Sábado, J., y Aradilla, A. (2011). Miedo a la muerte en estudiantes de enfermería. Enfermería Clínica, 16, 177-88.         [ Links ]

19. Hohman, Z. P., and Hogg, M. A. (2011). Fear and uncertainty in the face of death: The role of life after death in group identification. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41(6), 751-760. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.818.         [ Links ]

20. Izard, C. E. (2009). Emotion theory and research: Highlights, unanswered questions, and emerging issues. Annual review of psychology, 60, 1-25. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163539.         [ Links ]

21. Kastenbaum, R., and Heflick, N. A. (2011). Sad to say: is it time for Sorrow Management Theory?. OMEGA-Journal of Death and Dying, 62(4), 305-327. doi: 10.2190/OM.62.4.a.         [ Links ]

22. Lester, D., Templer, D.I., and Abdel-Khalek, A. (2007). A cross-cultural comparison of death anxiety: A brief note. Omega: Journal of Death and Dying, 54(3), 255-260. DOI:10.1080/07481187.2011.583200.         [ Links ]

23. Limonero, J.T., Tomás-Sábado, J., Fernández-Castro, J, Cladellas, R., and Gómez-Benito, J. (2010). Competencia personal percibida y ansiedad ante la muerte en estudiantes de enfermería. Ansiedad y estrés, 16(2-3), 177-88.         [ Links ]

24. Lincoln, Y. S., and Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Londres: Sage        [ Links ]

25. Liu, Y. C., Su, P. Y., Chen, C. H., Chiang, H. H., Wang, K. Y., & Tzeng, W. C. (2011). Facing death, facing self: nursing students' emotional reactions during an experiential workshop on life-and-death issues. Journal of clinical nursing, 20(5-6), 856-863. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2702.2010.03545.x.         [ Links ]

26. Lyke, J. (2013). Associations among aspects of meaning in life and death anxiety in young adults. Death studies, 37(5), 471-482. doi:10.1080/07481187.2011.649939.         [ Links ]

27. Martí-García, C., García-Caro, M. P., Schmidt-Riovalle, J., Fernández-Alcántara, M., Montoya-Juárez, R., y Cruz-Quintana, F. (2014). Formación en cuidados paliativos y efecto en la evaluación emocional de imágenes de muerte. Medicina Paliativa, doi:10.1016/j.medipa.2013.12.007.         [ Links ]

28. Mayring, P. (2000). Qualitative Content Analysis. Forum Qualitative Sozialforschung/Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 1(2). Retrieved from http://www.qualitative-research.net/ index.php/fqs/article/view/1089/2385.         [ Links ]

29. Mason, S. R., and Ellershaw, J. E. (2010). Undergraduate training in palliative medicine: Is more necessarily better? Palliative Medicine, 24(3), 306-309. doi: 10.1177/0269216309351867.         [ Links ]

30. Montoya-Juarez, R., García-Caro, M. P., Campos-Calderón, C., Schmidt-Rio Valle, J., Gomez-Chica, A., Marti-García, C., and Cruz-Quintana, F. (2013). Psychological responses of terminally ill patients who are experiencing suffering: A qualitative study. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 50(1), 53-62. doi:10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2012.08.016.         [ Links ]

31. Neimeyer, R A. (1994). Death anxiety handbook: Research, instrumentation, and application. Washington, D.C.: Taylor & Francis.         [ Links ]

32. Neimeyer, R. A. (2005). From death anxiety to meaning making at the end of life: Recommendations for psychological assessment. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 12(3), 354-357. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bpi036.         [ Links ]

33. Neimeyer, R. A., Currier, J. M., Coleman, R., Tomer, A., and Samuel, E. (2011). Confronting suffering and death at the end of life: The impact of religiosity, psychosocial factors, and life regret among hospice patients. Death Studies, 35(9), 777-800. doi:10.1080/07481187.2011.583200.         [ Links ]

34. Pineda Galán, C. (2012). Inteligencia Emocional y Bienestar Personal en estudiantes universitarios en ciencias de la salud. Tesis Doctoral. Universidad de Málaga. Disponible en http://riuma.uma.es/xmlui/browse?value=Pineda%20Gal%C3%A1n,%20Consolaci%C3%B3n&type=author#sthash.7l0B3ffc.dpuf.         [ Links ]

35. Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., and Maxfield, M. (2006). On the unique psychological import of the human awareness of mortality: Theme and variations. Psychological Inquiry, 17(4), 328-356. doi:10.1080/10478400701369542.         [ Links ]

36. Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., and Greenberg, J. (2015). Thirty Years of Terror Management Theory: From Genesis to Revelation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. doi: 10.1016/bs.aesp.2015.03.001.         [ Links ]

37. Rosenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., and Lyon, D. (1989). Evidence for terror management theory: I. The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who violate or uphold cultural values. Journal of personality and social psychology, 57(4), 681. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.681.         [ Links ]

38. Routledge, C., Juhl, J., and Vess, M. (2013). Mortality salience increases death-anxiety for individuals low in personal need for structure. Motivation and Emotion, 37(2), 303-307. doi: 10.1007/s11031-012-9313-6.         [ Links ]

39. Ryan, P.Y. (2005). Approaching death: a phenomenological study of five older adults with advanced cancer. Oncology Nursing Forum, 32, 1101-1108 doi: 10.1188/05.ONF.1101-1108.         [ Links ]

40. Sánchez Rueda, G. (2014) Las emociones en la práctica enfermera. Tesis doctoral. Universidad Autónoma de Barcelona. Disponible en http://hdl.handle.net/10803/284050.         [ Links ]

41. Schillerstrom, J. E., Sánchez-Reilly, S., and O'Donnell, L. (2012). Improving student comfort with death and dying discussions through facilitated family encounters. Academic Psychiatry, 36(3), 188-190. doi: 10.1176/appi.ap.10020032.         [ Links ]

42. Shih, F. J., Gau, M. L., Lin, Y. S., Pong, S. J., and Lin, H. R. (2006). Death and help expected from nurses when dying. Nursing Ethics, 13(4), 360-375. doi: 10.1191/0969733006ne881oa.         [ Links ]

43. Schmidt-RioValle, J., Montoya-Juárez, R., Campos-Calderon, C. P., García-Caro, M. P., Prados-Peña, D., y Cruz-Quintana, F. (2012). Efectos de un programa de formacion en cuidados paliativos sobre el afrontamiento de la muerte. Medicina Paliativa, 19(3), 113-120.         [ Links ]

44. Templer, D.I., Awadalla, A., Al-Fayez, G., Frazee, J., Bassman, L., Connelly, H. J., ... Abdel-Khalek, A. M. (2006). Construction of a death anxiety scale-extended. Omega: Journal of Death and Dying, 53(3), 209-226. doi: 10.2190/BQFP-9ULN-NULY-4JDR.         [ Links ]

45. Thomas, L.V. (1983). Antropología de la muerte. México D.F.: Fondo de Cultura Económica.         [ Links ]

46. Tomás-Sábado, J., y Benito, J. G. (2003). Variables relacionadas con la ansiedad ante la muerte. Revista de psicología general y aplicada: Revista de la Federación Española de Asociaciones de Psicología, 56(3), 257-279.         [ Links ]

47. van den Bos, K. (2009). Making sense of life: The existential self trying to deal with personal uncertainty. Psychological Inquiry, 20(4), 197-217. doi:10.1080/10478400903333411.         [ Links ]

48. Wanden-Berghe, C., Guardiola-Wanden-Berghe, R., y Sanz-Valero, J. (2009). Voluntades de la población sobre los cuidados y decisiones al final de la vida. Nutrición Hospitalaria, 24(6), 732-737.         [ Links ]

49. Zerpa, C. E. (2009). Sistemas emocionales y la tradición evolucionada en psicología. Summa Psicológica UST, 6(1), 113-123.         [ Links ]

 

 

Correspondence:
Celia Martí-García.
Universidad de Málaga.
Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud.
Departamento de Enfermería.
Arquitecto Francisco Peñalosa, 3,
29071 Málaga (Spain).
E-mail: celiamarti@uma.es

Article received: 18-01-2016
revised: 08-04-2016
accepted: 26-04-2016

Creative Commons License Todo o conteúdo deste periódico, exceto onde está identificado, está licenciado sob uma Licença Creative Commons