SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.29 número3Un Estudio sobre la competencia lectora en adultos con discapacidad intelectual y del desarrollo ante textos con contenidos de clínica y salud índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Revista

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • En proceso de indezaciónCitado por Google
  • No hay articulos similaresSimilares en SciELO
  • En proceso de indezaciónSimilares en Google

Compartir


Clínica y Salud

versión On-line ISSN 2174-0550versión impresa ISSN 1130-5274

Clínica y Salud vol.29 no.3 Madrid nov. 2018  Epub 02-Nov-2020

https://dx.doi.org/10.5093/clysa2018a16 

Articles

Social Cognition and Executive Function Impairment in Young Women with Anorexia Nervosa

La cognición social y el daño de la función ejecutiva en mujeres jóvenes que padecen anorexia nerviosa

Jonathan Adrián Zegarra-ValdiviaA  , Brenda Nadia Chino-Vilcaa  b 

aUniversidad Nacional de San Agustín de Arequipa, Perú

bUniversidad Católica San Pablo, Arequipa, Perú

ABSTRACT

Anorexia nervosa (AN) is a very debilitating disorder and has shown different cognitive deficit patterns. Some of them are controversial because they relate some deficit with autistic traits. Both, social cognition and executive function are top-down processes that regulate social interaction and adaptive behavior in a complex world. Neurocognitive profiles focused on this process are especially useful as endophenotypes in clinic research and intervention.The aims of this study are (1) assessing the contribution of social cognition and executive function to socio-emotional and neurocognitive patterns in anorexia nervosa and (2) investigating the possible relationships between social cognition tasks and executive function measures and clinic features in this eating disorder. Fifteen adolescents with anorexia nervosa and 15 healthy controls took part in this study. Patients were diagnosed with DSM-IV-R criteria. They had suffered the disease for 3 years (SD ± 1.47). They were compared in social cognition (Reading the Mind in the Eyes and the IOWA Gambling Task) and executive function (verbal fluency tasks, trail making test, and digit span test [DST] from the WAIS-III). The results show that there was a significant difference in social cognition, at the theory of mind and the IOWA gambling task. Finally, for the executive function task, adolescents with AN displayed significant differences in both direct DST and inverse DST, as well as in semantic fluency and phonological fluency compared with controls. These domains show different highly-significant relationships that could explain neurocognitive profiles. In conclusion, many differences were found between adolescents with AN and healthy controls. AN patients displayed important deficits in theory of mind recognition, executive function, and decision making. These deficits could play a role in maintaining pathological patterns and also important endophenotypes in research and intervention might be considered.

Keywords Anorexia nervosa; Endophenotypes; Eating disorders; Executive function; Social cognition

RESUMEN

La anorexia nerviosa es un trastorno muy debilitante y ha mostrado diferentes patrones de déficit cognitivo. Algunos de ellos son controvertidos porque se relacionan con rasgos autistas. Tanto la cognición social como la función ejecutiva son procesos de control “arriba/abajo” que regulan la interacción social y el comportamiento adaptativo en un mundo complejo. Los perfiles neurocognitivos enfocados en este proceso son especialmente útiles como endofenotipos en la investigación clínica y la intervención. Los objetivos de este estudio son: (1) evaluar la contribución de la cognición social y la función ejecutiva a los patrones socioemocionales y neurocognitivos en la anorexia nerviosa y (2) investigar las posibles relaciones entre las tareas de cognición social y las medidas de la función ejecutiva y las características clínicas de este trastorno de la conducta alimentaria. 15 adolescentes con anorexia nerviosa y 15 controles sanos participaron en este estudio. Los pacientes fueron diagnosticados con los criterios DSM-IV-R. Estos han sufrido la enfermedad durante al menos 3 años (SD ± 1,47). Fueron comparados en la cognición social (“Lectura de la mente a través de los ojos” y la tarea de juego IOWA) y función ejecutiva (tareas de fluidez verbal, prueba de seguimiento y prueba de spam digital del WAIS-III). Los resultados muestran diferencias significativas en la cognición social (teoría de la mente y en la tarea de juego de IOWA). Finalmente, para la tarea de la función ejecutiva, los adolescentes con AN mostraron diferencias significativas tanto en la DST directa como en la DST inversa, así como en la fluidez semántica y la fluidez fonológica. Estos dominios muestran diferentes relaciones altamente significativas que podrían explicar los perfiles neurocognitivos. En conclusión, se encontraron diferencias entre adolescentes con AN y controles sanos. Los pacientes mostraron déficits importantes en la teoría del reconocimiento de la mente, función ejecutiva y toma de decisiones. Estos déficits podrían desempeñar un papel en el mantenimiento de los patrones patológicos y también podrían considerarse endofenotipos importantes en la investigación y la intervención.

Palabras clave: Anorexia nerviosa; Endofenotipos; Trastornos de la conducta alimentaria; Función ejecutiva; Cognición social

Introduction

Anorexia Nervosa (AN) is a complex neuropsychiatric disorder with many interrelations involved in its development and maintenance (Abbate-Daga et al., 2011; Calderoni et al., 2013), such as environmental, familiar, cognitive, emotional, biological, and genetic vulnerability. AN is characterized by fear of gaining weight, inadequate intake, excessive weight loss, and body-image disturbances (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). In the last few years, different studies revealed common traits between the autistic spectrum disorder and AN, associating those symptoms with disruptions in basic neurocognitive processes, especially in social cognition and executive function. These alterations have been proposed as endophenotypes of AN and eating disorders (Hamatani, 2016; Kanakam, Raoult, Collier, & Treasure, 2013). Nevertheless, divergent aspects have been reported, and social cognition studies are in initial stage.

Different studies highlight alterations in executive function as key processes involved in etiopathogenesis, focused on the absence of inhibitory control (Claes, Nederkoorn, Vandereycken, Guerrieri, & Vertommen, 2006), cognitive flexibility (Roberts, Tchanturia, & Treasure, 2010), and perseveration (Waller et al., 2012). Additionally, set-shifting has shown to be and important impairment related to chronicity rather than specific endophenotypes (Vall & Wade, 2015); this would be supported by less specific studies, diagnostic specificity, or small groups. Moreover, no specific differences between healthy controls and individuals with AN were found (Vall & Wade, 2015), contrary to other studies (Chan et al., 2013; Lounes, Khan, & Tchanturia, 2011), pointing out a necessity of further analysis.

Impulsivity and decision-making have also been surveyed, being affected in AN patients. For example, they exhibit insensitivity to feedback in early stages of the IGT (Aloi et al., 2015), both restrictive and purgative AN, where decision-making looks like an interesting endophenotype of eating disorders (Guillaume et al., 2015; Tchanturia et al., 2007). This poor response reflects an alteration of ventromedial prefrontal structures, what leads to taking more risky decisions. On the other hand, clinical perfectionism has recently become an important trait, relevant in eating psychopathology, where a high level of perfectionism interfere with positive outcomes and the maintaining of AN (Hurst & Zimmer-Gembeck, 2015). This tendency to hold irrational expectations of others and oneself (Flett, Hewitt, Blankstein, & Koledin, 1991) may be supported by failures of the theory of mind, a key process to understanding others’ mind, emotions, and intentions (Baron-Cohen, Leslie, & Frith, 1985). Thus, some studies speculate about the relation of anorexia nervosa and autistic traits (Courty et al., 2013), underscoring deficits in empathic traits (Baron-Cohen et al., 2013) or social cognition skills (Renwick et al., 2015), even when compared with a new variance of Asperger’s syndrome (Oldershaw et al., 2011).

Specific deficits in neurocognitive processes such as the theory of mind, decision-making ,and executive function would point out endophenotype related-deficits for psychiatric disorders more than categorical relations. Highlighting neurocognitive networks as endophenotypes would elucidate genetic and neurobiological architecture deficits of the social brain. In summary, social cognition studies suggest an impairment in AN, but are still unclear or contradictory. Likewise, decision-making and executive function (EF) evidence have not been consistently studied in AN. Therefore, the aims of this study are (1) to assess the contribution of social cognition and executive function to socio-emotional and cognitive patterns in AN and (2) to investigate the possible relationships between social cognition tasks and EF measures and clinic features in AN.

Method

Participants

All participants were native Spanish speakers and were tested individually in a quiet room at the hospital. The sequence in which tests were administered were identical for all subjects. The demographic and clinical features of the sample are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of the Sample 

Note. 1Mean and standard deviation; 2by medical history;3from the first clinic diagnosis.

Instrument

Neuropsychological test. The neurocognitive appraisal was performed in two 30-minute sessions. We used the Reading the Mind in the Eyes (Baron-Cohen, Jolliffe, Mortimore, & Robertson, 1997; Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, Raste, & Plumb, 2001) and the IOWA Gambling Task (Bechara, 2004; Brevers, Bechara, Cleeremans, & Noël, 2013; Fukui, Murai, Fukuyama, Hayashi, & Hanakawa, 2005) to asses social cognition.

The Reading the Mind in the Eyes is a social sensitivity test (Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill et al., 2001; Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Spong, Scahill, & Lawson, 2001). Participants perceived 36 images of human faces expressing a complex mental state like thoughts, intentions, or emotions through facial expression. Each image was accompanied by four adjectives that described mental state. Participants were asked about the sexual identity of people in the pictures as a control perception task, in conjunction with the appropriate mental state (see Figure 1).

Figure 1 Reading the Mind in the Eyes. Chart illustrating the evaluation of gaze and sex recognition. Participants analyze images and choose one mental state out of four possible states. This test measures a person’s ability to understand others’ emotional states. 

The IOWA Gambling Task was supported by PEBL (Psychology Experimental Building Language; Mueller & Piper, 2014). It measures decision-making by risk-benefit elections following the selection of four possible cards. Participants receive rewards or penalizations in a total of 100 attempts; the assessed subject will know at all times how to make their decisions or not profits, and after 100 attempts the amount of successes/failures that mediated somatic markers (Bechara, 2004) were measured. An emotional learning curve with 5 principal scores of each 20 responses was designed (see Figure 2). The purpose of the game is to obtain the greatest possible reward (León, Martínez, Cruz, & Hernández, 2011).

Figure 2 IOWA Gambling Task. A screen shot of the test supported by PEBL. Participants must choose one card by one evoking aleatory rewards or punishment to obtain the major posible cash. A learning emotional curve is given after one hundred attempts. 

Other brief neurocognitive tests used to measure executive function were: the verbal fluency test, semantic and phonologic, and the digit span test. Both tests evaluated working memory, flexibility, and attentional shifting (Ardila & Ostrosky, 2012; Tirapu-Ustárroz, Muñoz-Céspedes, Pelegrín-Valero, & Albeniz-Ferreras, 2005; Wechsler, 1997). These tests were selected for its easy and administration and short time of administration.

Ethical Statement

Neurocognitive appraisal was conducted with the approval of the Research Ethics Board of the “Moisés Heresi” Hospital in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki. Informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Data Analysis

The statistical analysis was performed with SPSS version 20 (SPSS, Inc., USA). We used the Mann-Whitney U-test to compare both groups. We also used the Spearman correlation between variables. Table 2 displays the Spearman correlations. Results were significant at p < .05 and p < .01.

Results

The theory of mind and decision-making are core cognitive processes in social cognition. The theory of mind appraisal does not find any difference for recognition of sexual identity (u = 76.000, p < .117) as a control measure of perception process. Gaze recognition of intentions, thoughts, and emotion display statistically significant differences between AN patients and healthy controls (u = 56.000, p < .018) (see Figure 3). The theory of mind is clearly reduced in the clinic group compared with healthy controls and could be related with less social interaction or empathy and more autistic characteristics. The absence of differences in sex recognition display a preserved perception process. More importantly, the mental state essay may show failures to understand others’ mind and will be and indirect measure of their own comprehension of mental states and emotions in these subjects.

Figure 3 Reading the Mind in the Eyes. Comparison between clinic and control group. Patients with anorexia nervosa display worse recognition of gaze than controls. Perhaps, they display a normal sex recognition. 

The decision-making essay was divided in five scores (Bechara, 2004): the first one, IOWA 1, obtained with the first 20 items, does not show discrepancy between groups (u = 78.000, p < .150); the second one, IOWA 2 (items 21-40), shows significant differences (u = 63.000, p < .038); IOWA 3 (items 41-60) and IOWA 4 (items 61-80) scores do not display any difference (u = 93.000, p < .411) and (u = 88.000, p < .304) between both groups respectively; the last score, IOWA 5 (items 81-100) exhibits statistically significant differences (u = 47.000, p < .006). Figure 4 displays an emotional-learning curve in this task with five scores. Healthy participants take more advantageous choices over time consistent with feedback across all elections. By contrast, participants with AN take some disadvantageous choices and their feedback does not show an “adaptive” effect on a better selection. Over time, patients are more impulsive, take risky elections, and will be related with poor clinic response.

Figure 4 IOWA Gambling Task. The clinic group shows significant differences in IOWA scores 2 and 5 compared with controls. They take more disadvantage decisions showing more impulsivity than controls. 

For the executive function (see Figures 5 and 6), direct (u = 58.000, p < .022) and inverse (u = 34.000, p < .001) digit-span test shows significant differences together with verbal fluency, in semantic (u = 52.500, p < .012) and phonologic (u = 63.500, p < .04) tasks.

Figure 5 Digit Span Test. The clinic group shows significant differences in both (direct and inverse) task, that displays working memory deficits. 

Figure 6 verbal Fluency. The clinic group shows significant differences in verbal fluency patterns and lexical access with worse semantic and phonological fluency than controls. 

Table 2 displays the Spearman correlations. The duration of illness correlate negatively with gaze recognition (p ≤ .033), IOWA 2 (p ≤.006), IOWA 5 (p ≤ .012), and the executive function tasks, such as direct span (p ≤ .015), inverse span (p ≤ .000), phonological (p ≤ .037) and semantic (p ≤ .006) fluency. Other significant relations were not found. Nonetheless, gaze recognition of theory of mind showed positive correlates with inverse span (p ≤ .004) and semantic fluency (p ≤ .022) scores.

Table 2 Spearman Correlation 

*p < .05, **p ≤ .01.

The gambling task shows correlations between scores, as well as digit-span test and verbal fluency (see Table 2). Nonetheless, IOWA 3 correlates with direct span (p ≤ .033*), IOWA 4 with inverse span (p ≤ .002*), phonological fluency (p ≤ .013*) and semantic fluency (p ≤ .022*). Results in the gambling task may reflect a dependent executive function activity (i.e., in more accumulative items in time from 41 to 100 as a working memory mediation).

Discussion

The aim of this study was to evaluate the neuropsychological profile of young people with anorexia nervosa, and compare them with a healthy control group, appraising the social cognition and executive function cognitive processes, and analyzing how they are related. The main result is that young people with anorexia nervosa are characterized by worse social cognition abilities and executive function in comparison with healthy controls. Besides, moderate correlations between domains were found. Those disturbances would be understood as endophenotypes of the disorder.

Regarding social cognition, the theory of mind and decision-making were assessed. For the first one, AN shows a normal recognition of sex as a control perceptual task. In contrast, AN patients are less proficient in recognition of intentions, thoughts, and emotions. These results would be related with higher autistic phenotypes (Calderoni et al., 2013; Courty et al., 2013; De Sampaio et al., 2013) showing a poorer analysis of social stimuli, reducing skills to integrate social stimulation and communicative skills, etc. (Kasperek-Zimowska, Zimowski, Biernacka, Kucharska, & Rybakowski, 2016). This process represents a complex control of socio-emotional monitoring, its handicap being that it would be less stable and worse than other disorders such as schizophrenia or the autism spectrum disorder. In a recent meta-analysis study, Westwood et al., (2016) report a significant prevalence of autistic traits in AN patients, although etiology of this deficits is unknown.

Further research may clarify if this trait is a feature or a state (Baron-Cohen et al., 2013). For example, studies need to focus on non-affected relatives searching for similar mentalizing patterns, as well as longitudinal studies and more specific selection of participants with the same age, illness duration, and symptoms. In decision-making, our study reflects similar results as previous studies (Guillaume et al., 2015; Tchanturia et al., 2007), that reflect poorer sensitivity to somatic markers and feedback, alteration of ventromedial prefrontal area, and greater need for immediate reward with an alteration of motivational processes involved in decision-making (Bechara, 2004; Damasio, 1994). These patterns seem to be specific of AN and would be important for detailed therapy/rehabilitation (Aloi et al., 2015; Matsumoto et al., 2015).

Nonetheless, social cognition might involve a neural system related with consciousness as a cognitive and socio-emotional monitoring explaining different symptoms in AN, and its impairment could be considered as an endophenotype of this disorder. Specifically, in AN, hypoactivation in social-cognitive brain networks was reported, involving temporal and medial prefrontal cortex, orbito-medial prefrontal cortex, and anterior cingulate activity alterations (Oltra-Cucarella, Espert-Tortajada, & Rojo-Moreno, 2012; Schulte-Rüther, Mainz, Fink, Herpertz-Dahlmann, & Konrad, 2012; Tenconi et al., 2015).

In the executive function, patients with AN exhibit worse performance compared to healthy controls. Besides, the executive function reflects a complex conjunction of cognitive processes such as verbal fluency, attention, working memory, planning, etc. Our results display similar differences with controls as previous studies (Calderoni et al., 2013; Fagundo et al., 2012; Gillberg et al., 2010; Oltra-Cucarella et al., 2012; Oltra-Cucarella et al., 2015; Zakzanis, Campbell, & Polsinelli, 2010). Alterations in the executive function in anorexia nervosa are well documented, exhibiting profound dysfunctions in prefrontal and cortico-subcortical networks. Other neurobiological correlates involve ventral attention network, fronto-parietal networks, thalamus, caudate nucleus, and putamen (Collantoni et al., 2016; Lao-Kaim et al., 2015; Oltra-Cucarella et al., 2012).

As for the relationships between domains, Spearman correlations between the theory of mind and digit span test and semantic verbal fluency are moderate. This would be mediated by some similar mechanisms, as working memory and language, as well as attention networks implicated in both domains (Perner & Lang, 1999). Furthermore, emotion-based decision-making scores correlate with executive function displayed by similar neural mechanisms (Fagundo et al., 2012; Homberg, 2012). On the other hand, illness duration show a negative significant correlation with ToM gaze score, as well as with the executive function test (working memory/verbal fluency), probably indicating an impaired condition of those cognitive processes, regardless of the pharmacological/psychotherapy interventions.

The neuropsychological profiles restrict functional and structural processes involved as endophenotypes with neurobiological and genetic disturbances in neuropsychiatric disorders. Given the potential importance of this subject, it is necessary to further investigate the complicated relationship between central coherence, executive function, and social cognition with more specific neurobiological approaches, as fRMI studies, measures of serum neurotrophic factors like BDNF, insulin-like growth factor, oxytocin, vasopressin, leptine, etc. (Dmitrzak-Weglarz et al., 2013; Kasperek-Zimowska et al., 2016; Monteleone & Maj, 2013; Phillips, Jimerson, Pillai, & Wolfe, 2016), or more specific animal models linking biological, behavioral, and cognitive methods.

Limitations

The main limitation of this study was the small sample size of patients and the absence of AN sub-type groups, such as restrictive or purgative groups. Also a lack of complete neuropsychological evaluation of potential processes, such as central coherence, the cognitive theory of mind, or extending neuropsychological batteries in executive function to evaluate more subdomains, and preferentially with ecological tests (Zoo Test, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, etc.). Additionally, a follow-up study, and the inclusion of serum measures, sub-clinic, and kin participants would help to understand AN endophenotypes and improve neuropsychological interventions.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the participants in this study.

Cite this article as: Zegarra-Valdivia, J. A. & Chino-Vilca, B. N. (2018). Social cognition and executive function impairment in young women with anorexia nervosa. Clínica y Salud, 29, 107-113. https://doi.org/10.5093/clysa2018a16

References

Abbate-Daga, G., Buzzichelli, S., Amianto, F., Rocca, G., Marzola, E., McClintock, S. M., & Fassino, S. (2011). Cognitive flexibility in verbal and nonverbal domains and decision making in anorexia nervosa patients: a pilot study. BMC Psychiatry, 11(1), 162. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-11-162 [ Links ]

Aloi, M., Rania, M., Caroleo, M., Bruni, A., Palmieri, A., Cauteruccio, M. A., … Segura-García, C. (2015). Decision making, central coherence and set-shifting: a comparison between binge eating disorder, anorexia nervosa, and healthy controls. BMC Psychiatry, 15(1), 6. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-015-0395-z [ Links ]

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). DSM-V. American Journal of Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.744053 [ Links ]

Ardila, A., & Ostrosky, F. (2012). Guía para el diagnóstico neuropsicológico. Instituto de Neurociencia. Retrieved from http://ineuro.cucba.udg.mx/libros/bv_guia_para_el_diagnostico_neuropsicologico.pdfLinks ]

Baron-Cohen, S., Jaffa, T., Davies, S., Auyeung, B., Allison, C., & Wheelwright, S. (2013). Do girls with anorexia nervosa have elevated autistic traits? Molecular Autism, 4, 24. https://doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-4-24 [ Links ]

Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 38, 813-822. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01599.x [ Links ]

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, a M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37-46. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8 [ Links ]

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 42, 241-251. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00715 [ Links ]

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Spong, A., Scahill, V., & Lawson, J. (2001). Are intuitive physics and intuitive psychology independent ? A test with children with Asperger Syndrome. Learning, 5, 47-78. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.330/supp-1” 10.7717/peerj.330/supp-1 [ Links ]

Bechara, A. (2004). The role of emotion in decision-making: Evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 30-40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2003.04.001 [ Links ]

Brevers, D., Bechara, A., Cleeremans, A., & Noël, X. (2013). Iowa Gambling Task (IGT): twenty years after - gambling disorder and IGT. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 665. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00665 [ Links ]

Calderoni, S., Muratori, F., Leggero, C., Narzisi, A., Apicella, F., Balottin, U., … Urgesi, C. (2013). Neuropsychological functioning in children and adolescents with restrictive-type anorexia nervosa: an in-depth investigation with NEPSY-II. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 35, 167-179. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2012.760536 [ Links ]

Chan, T. W. S., Ahn, W.-Y., Bates, J. E., Busemeyer, J. R., Guillaume, S., Redgrave, G. W., … Courtet, P. (2013). Differential impairments underlying decision making in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: A cognitive modeling analysis. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 47, 157-167. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22223 [ Links ]

Claes, L., Nederkoorn, C., Vandereycken, W., Guerrieri, R., & Vertommen, H. (2006). Impulsiveness and lack of inhibitory control in eating disorders. Eating Behaviors, 7, 196-203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2006.05.001 [ Links ]

Collantoni, E., Michelon, S., Tenconi, E., Degortes, D., Titton, F., Manara, R., … Favaro, A. (2016). Functional connectivity correlates of response inhibition impairment in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Research - Neuroimaging, 247(2016), 9-16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.11.008 [ Links ]

Courty, A., Maria, A. S., Lalanne, C., Ringuenet, D., Vindreau, C., Chevallier, C., … Berthoz, S. (2013). Levels of autistic traits in anorexia nervosa: a comparative psychometric study. BMC Psychiatry, 13, 1. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-13-222 [ Links ]

Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ error and the future of human life. Scientific American, 271, 144. https://doi.org/ 10.1038/scientificamerican1094-144 [ Links ]

De Sampaio, F. T. P., Soneira, S., Aulicino, A., Martese, G., Iturry, M., & Allegri, R. F. (2013). Theory of mind and central coherence in eating disorders: Two sides of the same coin? Psychiatry Research, 210, 1116-1122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.08.051 [ Links ]

Dmitrzak-Weglarz, M., Skibinska, M., Slopien, A., Tyszkiewicz, M., Pawlak, J., MacIukiewicz, M., … Hauser, J. (2013). Serum neurotrophin concentrations in polish adolescent girls with anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychobiology, 67, 25-32. https://doi.org/10.1159/000343500 [ Links ]

Fagundo, A. B., de la Torre, R., Jiménez-Murcia, S., Agüera, Z., Granero, R., Tárrega, S., … Fernández-Aranda, F. (2012). Executive functions profile in extreme eating/weight conditions: From anorexia nervosa to obesity. PLoS ONE, 7(8). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0043382 [ Links ]

Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., Blankstein, K. R., & Koledin, S. (1991). Dimensions of perfectionism and irrational thinking. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 9, 185-201. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01061229 [ Links ]

Fukui, H., Murai, T., Fukuyama, H., Hayashi, T., & Hanakawa, T. (2005). Functional activity related to risk anticipation during performance of the Iowa gambling task. NeuroImage, 24, 253-259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.028 [ Links ]

Gillberg, I. C., Billstedt, E., Wentz, E., Anckarsäter, H., Råstam, M., & Gillberg, C. L. (2010). Attention, executive functions, and mentalizing in anorexia nervosa eighteen years after onset of eating disorder. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32, 358-365. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390903066857 [ Links ]

Guillaume, S., Gorwood, P., Jollant, F., Van den Eynde, F., Courtet, P., & Richard-Devantoy, S. (2015). Impaired decision-making in symptomatic anorexia and bulimia nervosa patients: a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 45, 3377-3391. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171500152X [ Links ]

Hamatani, S., Tomotake, M., Takeda, T., Kameoka, M., Kawabata, M., Kubo, H., … Ohmori, T. (2016). Impaired social cognition in anorexia nervosa patients. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 12, 2527-2531. https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S116521 [ Links ]

Homberg, J. R. (2012). Serotonin and decision making processes. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 218-236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.06.001 [ Links ]

Hurst, K., & Zimmer-Gembeck, M. (2015). Focus on perfectionism in female adolescent anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 48, 936-941. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22417 [ Links ]

Kanakam, N., Raoult, C., Collier, D., & Treasure, J. (2013). Set shifting and central coherence as neurocognitive endophenotypes in eating disorders: A preliminary investigation in twins. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 14, 464-475. https://doi.org/10.3109/15622975.2012.665478 [ Links ]

Kasperek-Zimowska, B., Zimowski, J., Biernacka, K., Kucharska, K., & Rybakowski, F. (2016). Impaired social cognition processes in Asperger syndrome and anorexia nervosa. in search for endophenotypes of social cognition. Psychiatria Polska, 50, 533-542. https://doi.org/10.12740/PP/OnlineFirst/33485 [ Links ]

Lao-Kaim, N. P., Fonville, L., Giampietro, V. P., Williams, S. C. R., Simmons, A., & Tchanturia, K. (2015). Aberrant function of learning and cognitive control networks underlie inefficient cognitive flexibility in anorexia nervosa: A cross-sectional fMRI study. PLoS ONE, 10(5), 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124027 [ Links ]

León, F. G., Martínez, J. M. A., Cruz, J. S., & Hernández, L. M. (2011). Emoción y Toma de Decisiones: Teoría y Aplicación de la IOWA. Revista Electrónica de Psicología Iztacala, 14, 333-353. Retrieved from www.iztacala.unam.mx/carreras/psicologia/psiclin%5Cnwww.revistas.unam.mx/index.php/repi [ Links ]

Lounes, N., Khan, G., & Tchanturia, K. (2011). Assessment of cognitive flexibility in anorexia nervosa--self-report or experimental measure? A brief report. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 925-928. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000671 [ Links ]

Matsumoto, J., Hirano, Y., Numata, N., Matzuzawa, D., Murano, S., Yokote, K., … Nakazato, M. (2015). Comparison in decision-making between bulimia nervosa, anorexia nervosa, and healthy women: influence of mood status and pathological eating concerns. Journal of Eating Disorders, 3, 14. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-015-0050-6 [ Links ]

Monteleone, P., & Maj, M. (2013). Dysfunctions of leptin, ghrelin, BDNF and endocannabinoids in eating disorders: Beyond the homeostatic control of food intake. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38, 312-330. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.10.021 [ Links ]

Mueller, S. T., & Piper, B. J. (2014). The Psychology Experiment Building Language (PEBL) and PEBL Test Battery. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 222, 250-259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.024 [ Links ]

Oldershaw, A., Hambrook, D., Stahl, D., Tchanturia, K., Treasure, J., & Schmidt, U. (2011). The socio-emotional processing stream in Anorexia Nervosa. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 970-988. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.11.001 [ Links ]

Oltra Cucarella, J., Espert Tortajada, R., & Rojo Moreno, L. (2012). Neuropsychology and anorexia nervosa. Cognitive and radiological findings. Neurología (Barcelona, Spain), 27, 504-510. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nrl.2011.08.003 [ Links ]

Oltra-Cucarella, J., Rojo Moreno, L., Arribas Sáiz, P., Sanguesa García, C., Latorre Paniagua, P., Hidalgo Muñoz, E., & Rojo Bofill, L. (2015). Impaired executive functioning influences verbal memory in anorexia nervosa. Eating Behaviors, 16, 47-53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.10.013 [ Links ]

Perner, J., & Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 337-344. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01362-5 [ Links ]

Phillips, K. E., Jimerson, D. C., Pillai, A., & Wolfe, B. E. (2016). Plasma BDNF levels following weight recovery in anorexia nervosa. Physiology and Behavior, 165(2016), 300-303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.08.014 [ Links ]

Renwick, B., Musiat, P., Lose, A., Dejong, H., Broadbent, H., Kenyon, M., … Schmidt, U. (2015). Neuro- and social-cognitive clustering highlights distinct profiles in adults with anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 48, 26-34. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22366 [ Links ]

Roberts, M. E., Tchanturia, K., & Treasure, J. L. (2010). Exploring the neurocognitive signature of poor set-shifting in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 44, 964-970. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.03.001 [ Links ]

Schulte-Rüther, M., Mainz, V., Fink, G. R., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., & Konrad, K. (2012). Theory of mind and the brain in anorexia nervosa: Relation to treatment outcome. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, 832-841. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.06.007 [ Links ]

Tchanturia, K., Liao, P. C., Uher, R., Lawrence, N., Treasure, J., & Campbell, I. C. (2007). An investigation of decision making in anorexia nervosa using the Iowa Gambling Task and skin conductance measurements. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 635-641. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617707070798 [ Links ]

Tenconi, E., Degortes, D., Clementi, M., Collantoni, E., Pinato, C., Forzan, M., … Favaro, A. (2015). Clinical and genetic correlates of decision making in anorexia nervosa. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 3395, 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2015.1112878 [ Links ]

Tirapu-Ustárroz, J., Muñoz-Céspedes, J.M., Pelegrín-Valero, C., & Albeniz-Ferreras, A. (2005). Propuesta de un protocolo para la evaluación de las funciones ejecutivas. Revista de Neurología, 41(3), 177-186. https://www.neurologia.com/articulo/2005054Links ]

Vall, E., & Wade, T. D. (2015). Trail making task performance in inpatients with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review : The Journal of the Eating Disorders Association, 23, 304-311. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2364 [ Links ]

Waller, G., Shaw, T., Meyer, C., Haslam, M., Lawson, R., & Serpell, L. (2012). Persistence, perseveration and perfectionism in the eating disorders. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 40, 462-473. https://doi.org/10.1017/S135246581200015X [ Links ]

Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS-III administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191102009001003 [ Links ]

Westwood, H., Eisler, I., Mandy, W., Leppanen, J., Treasure, J., & Tchanturia, K. (2016). Using the autism spectrum quotient to measure traits in anorexia nervosa: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 964-977. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2641-0 [ Links ]

Zakzanis, K. K., Campbell, Z., & Polsinelli, A. (2010). Quantitative evidence for distinct cognitive impairment in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Neuropsychology, 4, 89-106. https://doi.org/10.1348/174866409X459674 [ Links ]

Recibido: 06 de Mayo de 2018; Aprobado: 12 de Julio de 2018

Correspondence: adrianzegarra13@gmail.com (A. Zegarra-Valdivia).

Conflict of Interest

The authors of this article declare no conflict of interest.

Creative Commons License This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial No Derivative License, which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium provided the original work is properly cited and the work is not changed in any way.