Introduction
As indicated by Fernández-Montalvo and Echeburúa (2006), it is increasingly difficult to find so-called “pure” clinical cases, i.e., patients who are afflicted by only one psychopathological disorder, as new research is showing that there is comorbidity between the different disorders. This fact implies a new approach to the evaluation and explanation of such disorders, and, consequently, the need to analyse and adapt the tools used for evaluating and explaining them. It further indicates the need for the creation of new tools that enable the gathering of more accurate information about the patient's disorders and its possible treatment.
Despite the fact that numerous measures have been developed to evaluate personality disorders in recent years, and against the opinions of some authors (Dutton, 2003; Gondolf, 2003, among others), self-report methods (questionnaires, inventories, scales) remain the most commonly used measurement tools. These methods can produce skewed results because of the ease of manipulating, simulating or even hiding symptoms. Because of this, the reliability of the results and the validity of any inferences made are subject to serious questioning (Calcedo, 2000; Echeburúa, Amor, & Corral, 2003).
On the other hand, the results obtained from different measurement instruments used in the diagnosis of these types of disorders often show low levels of agreement, which means that inferences and conclusions drawn from these data are also questionable (Hyler et al., 1989; Zimmerman & Coryell, 1990). In any case, a clinical diagnosis must be based on information obtained by various means: self-reporting, interviews, etc.
In the field of psychopathology, self-reporting has been used for both the evaluation of specific disorders (Fernández-Montalvo & Echeburúa, 2006) and the exploration of symptomatology, being it general or specific to an individual. One of the self-reporting methods most commonly used for the latter is the SCL-90 (Derogatis, 1977; Derogatis & Cleary, 1977; Derogatis, Lipman, & Covi, 1973), composed of 90 items across nine different scales that seek to measure nine basic dimensions: somatisation, obsessive compulsion, interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxiety, hostility, phobic anxiety, paranoid ideation and psychoticism. Derogatis and Savitz (1999) developed an updated version of the scale (SCL90-R) 90-item, multidimensional self-report symptom inventory which measures symptomatic psychological distress. The SCL-90-R results in three global measures of distress as well as the nine primary symptom dimensions defined before and did not represent a significant change respect to the SCL-90 since only two items were modified. However, the SCL-90 as well as the updated version, presents a few problems that complicate its use: the large number of items, the overlap in items, the high correlation among the nine scales, the low degree of discriminant validity, the instability of its structure, and the lack of factorial validity, indicated by the fact that factorial structures that varied from one to ten factors were proposed in various studies of the questionnaire (Davison et al., 1997; Sandín et al., 2008; Vassend & Skrondal, 1999). In order to avoid some of these inconveniences, Davison et al. (1997) developed a questionnaire, the Symptom Assessment-45 Questionnaire (SA-45), that (a) maintained the advantages of the SCL-90; (b) required only half the number of items used in the SCL-90 (45 items); (c) contained the same number of items for each scale, in order to retain their reliability; and (d) avoided item overlap, thus minimising correlations among the different scales by increasing its discriminant validity and favouring the utility and usability of the instrument. This last improvement is especially important given the existing efforts to integrate the treatment of behavioural problems into primary care services, in order to offer more efficient and effective service to individuals with psychological and emotional problems.
Maruish, Bershadsky, & Goldstein (1998) carried out a project designed to demonstrate the benefits of integrating behavioral healthcare services in primary medical care settings providing an opportunity to further investigate the psychometric properties of the SA45 using data from a sample of 126 adults. Specifically, the appropriateness of the SA45's adult nonpatient norms, as well as cross-validation of its test-retest reliability and construct validity, was investigated. The results suggested that use of the SA45 nonpatient norms with primary care populations is appropriate. The use of the SA-45 has demonstrated its utility in (a) identifying and classifying psychiatric patients, although accurate classification of patients requires further evaluation for behavioral health problems, (b) evaluating psychopathological changes in patients with health problems, and (c) highlighting the effects of psychological treatment in psychiatric patients.
The psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the SA-45 have been studied by Sandín et al. (2008) using a sample of 420 students from various public universities in Madrid. The main objective of his work was the study of the factorial structure of the questionnaire through the implementation of a series of factor analyses, both exploratory and confirmatory. The results of the exploratory factor analyses suggest a factorial structure of nine factors, which was later corroborated through a confirmatory factor analysis. A similar study was carried out by Alvarado, Sandín, Valdez-Medina, González-Arraia, & Rivera (2012) using a sample of 418 university students from the Universidad Autónoma del Estado de México. The results showed that the psychometric characteristics of the questionnaire were very similar even when dealing with samples from diverse cultural backgrounds. However, factorial solutions are difficult to replicate, even under exceptionally favourable circumstances as the exploratory factor analysis is sensitive to the characteristics and size of the sample group used (Costello & Osborne, 2005; Osborne, Costello, & Kellow, 2008; Osborne & Fitzpatrick, 2012).
As the study by Sandín et al. (2008) was the first study in which the Spanish version of the SA-45 was validated psychometrically, we have deemed it beneficial to carry out a new study. The main objectives of this new study are to analyse the internal structure of SA-45 and to evaluate its reliability in a non-clinical sample.
Method
Participants
The sample was composed of 823 university students from the National University of Distance Education of Spain, with 21.8 % of the sample being male and 78.2 % being female. The differences in percentage between male and female subjects can be explained by the fact that the sampling was incidental and it is the nature of the population of students in this university. The average age in the group was 36.8 (SD = 10.1), with an average of 38.9 (SD = 10.7) for males and 36.2 (SD = 9.9) for females. Participants ranged from 19 to 52 years of age. However, this age and gender differences between the two groups was not statistically significant. All participants were informed of the nature and purpose of the experiment and all gave their written informed consent.
Measures
The Spanish version of the SA-45 (Symptom Assessment-45 Questionnaire; Davison et al., 1997), created by Sandín et al. (2008), was used in this study. It is a questionnaire comprising 45 items on a five-level Likert scale with items ranging from 0 (“Not at all”) to 4 (“Very much so, or extremely”). The questionnaire derives from the original Symptom Checklist-90 (SCL-90; Derogatis, Lipman, & Covi, 1973) and seeks to evaluate the following dimensions: somatisation, obsessive compulsion, interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxiety, hostility, phobic anxiety, paranoid ideation and psychoticism.
The questionnaire was developed, primarily, as a tool to evaluate the results obtained from different treatments applied to psychiatric patients (Davison et al., 1997), but studies are being carried out with non-clinical sample groups as a means of determining the questionnaire's potential usefulness in detecting the level of overall mental health of participants and/or the presence of any indication of psychological pathology or maladjustment. The questionnaire was implemented and answered on-line.
Procedure
Participants were recruited through a personal e-mail addressed sent to students enrolled in various courses taught. They were informed that their participation would be voluntary and anonymous and would consist of filling out a questionnaire posted on a website accessed with a username and password, both of which could be verified as authentic by the university system.
Statistical analysis
Firstly, the analyses carried out focused on the subjects' responses to each item and the distribution of these responses across the different categories. Secondly, different factor analyses were carried out in two sub-samples, both exploratory (N = 413) and confirmatory (N = 410), to analyse which factorial structure presented the best goodness of fit. Thirdly, the reliability of the questionnaire was analysed using Cronbach's coefficient alpha, both at a global level and at the levels of each of the postulated scales.
Given the characteristic of the data, that is ordinal variables, we decided to used minimum unweighted least square and promax rotation, with polychoric correlation as they are considered to be the most adequate procedure (Barense, Oort, & Timmerman, 2015). In the same sense, in order to take into account the nature of the data, and prevent the assumptions' violation of the Confirmatory Factor Analysis, we used unweighted least square as estimation method (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1989; Holgado-Tello, Chacón, Barbero, & Vila, 2010; Holgado-Tello, Morata-Ramírez, & Barbero, 2018; Morata-Ramírez, Holgado-Tello, Barbero-García, & Méndez, 2015; Yang-Wallentin, Jöreskog & Luo, 2010). The analyses were carried out using SPSS (version 15) and LISREL (version 8.8).
Results
Analysis of the responses
The distributions obtained for each of the items highlighted their skewness and lack of variability. The mode for all the items fell on 0 (the category “Not at all”). The median also fell on 0 for all items except numbers 5, 10, 12 and 18, where it fell on “Somewhat present”. The means oscillated between 0.03 and 0.96 and the standard deviations were between 0.22 and 1.07.
For many of the items, over 90 % of the responses fall into the “Not at all” category, and for many of the statements, none of the respondents selected the last two categories. The skewness of the responses, as well as the lack of discrimination of the statements, will affect the results of later analyses.
Exploratory factor analysis of S-45
The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin test (= 0.923) and Bartlett's sphericity test (χ2(990) = 9389.152, p = 0.000) highlighted the suitability of the data for factor analysis.
The results obtained seem to confirm a factorial structure of nine factors that explain 62.82% of the variance. However, the first factor itself accounts for 32.87 % of the variance; the next factor accounting for just 5.86 %. The eigenvalues obtained from the nine factors were as follows (in parentheses after each figure is the variance percentage explained by the corresponding eigenvalue): 14.79 (32.87 %); 2.64 (5.86 %); 2.25 (5.01 %); 2.02 (4.49 %); 1.67 (3.71 %); 1.34 (2.99 %); 1.24 (2.76 %); 1.23 (2.73 %); and 1.08 (2.40 %). This difference in the variance percentage between the first factor and the remaining factors leads us to consider the existence of a basic dominant factor. The correlations between the factors reached 0.72. On the other hand, a large majority of items have been found to load in more than one factor with factor weights of over 0.5, which indicates the existence of overlap between items. Laid out below are the results obtained when the items are assigned to the different factors according to factor saturation. Table 1 shows the results obtained by assuming that an item should form part of a factor when its factor saturation within the factor itself is greater than or equal to 0.5. However, three items are included whose factor saturation did not reach 0.5 in any of the factors. The last column shows the scale that, in theory, and according to Sandín et al. (2008) work, should have an item assigned to it.
Items | F 1 | F 2 | F 3 | F 4 | F 5 | F 6 | F 7 | F 8 | F 9 | Scale |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
14 | .794 | .571 | .667 | .576 | .538 | IS | ||||
44 | .757 | PI | ||||||||
15 | .697 | .621 | IS | |||||||
19 | .662 | PI | ||||||||
40 | .658 | PI | ||||||||
27 | .620 | .562 | DE | |||||||
11 | .608 | .524 | .609 | DE | ||||||
2 | .571 | .521 | PI | |||||||
5 | .543 | PI | ||||||||
3 | .774 | PA | ||||||||
22 | .758 | PA | ||||||||
24 | .730 | PA | ||||||||
37 | .708 | PA | ||||||||
8 | .685 | PA | ||||||||
38 | .538 | AN | ||||||||
6 | .463 | AN | ||||||||
26 | .750 | SO | ||||||||
29 | .713 | SO | ||||||||
31 | .685 | SO | ||||||||
18 | .620 | SO | ||||||||
23 | .603 | SO | ||||||||
25 | .550 | OC | ||||||||
43 | .816 | HO | ||||||||
7 | .729 | HO | ||||||||
39 | .544 | .711 | HO | |||||||
35 | .671 | .544 | HO | |||||||
34 | .467 | HO | ||||||||
42 | .564 | .540 | .804 | .647 | DE | |||||
17 | .506 | .773 | IS | |||||||
36 | .573 | .513 | .717 | IS | ||||||
32 | .635 | .655 | IS | |||||||
4 | .654 | PS | ||||||||
33 | .598 | PS | ||||||||
13 | .553 | PI | ||||||||
1 | .335 | PS | ||||||||
30 | .550 | .595 | .500 | .848 | .537 | AN | ||||
41 | .560 | .529 | .574 | .526 | .812 | .532 | AN | |||
12 | .513 | .504 | .523 | .800 | AN | |||||
10 | .566 | .548 | .624 | .740 | DE | |||||
9 | .617 | .522 | .554 | .699 | DE | |||||
28 | .540 | .544 | .556 | .553 | .676 | .577 | OC | |||
45 | .518 | PS | ||||||||
16 | .772 | OC | ||||||||
20 | .721 | OC | ||||||||
21 | .582 | .537 | .587 | OC |
Note.IS=interpersonal sensitivity; DE=depression; PI=paranoid ideation; PA=phobic anxiety; AN=anxiety; SO=somatisation; HO=hostility; PS=psychoticism; OC=obsessive compulsion.
Of the 45 items that comprise the questionnaire, there are 21 that have loadings over 0.5 in the first factor. On the other hand, there are several cross-loading items. Analysing the composition of the factors in accordance with the categorisation proposals made by Sandín et al. (2008), the first factor would include the following scales: paranoid ideation (2, 5, 19, 40, and 44), depression (9, 10, 11, 27, and 42), interpersonal sensitivity (14, 15, 17, 32, and 36), three of the five items on the anxiety scale (12, 31 and 41), an item from the hostility scale (39) and two items from obsessive compulsion (21 and 28). It could be classed as a general factor of emotional maladjustment. The second factor would comprise the five items corresponding to the hypothetical scale of phobic anxiety (3, 8, 22, 24, and 37) along with two items assigned to the anxiety scale (6 and 38) and an item belonging to the scale of interpersonal sensitivity. The third factor would comprise items corresponding to the scale known as somatisation (18, 23, 26, 29, and 31) along with three items corresponding to the anxiety scale (12, 30, and 41) and two items from the scale of obsessive compulsion (25 and 28). The fourth factor includes the items from the hostility scale (7, 34, 35, 39, and 41) along with three items from the depression scale (9, 10, and 42), three from anxiety (12, 30, and 41), one from paranoid ideation (2), one from obsessive compulsion (28) and one from interpersonal sensitivity (14). Factor 5 is comprised of the five items on the scale of interpersonal sensibility (14, 15, 17, 32, and 36) along with three items from depression (10, 11, and 42), one from the anxiety scale (41), one from obsessive compulsion (21) and two from paranoid ideation (19 and 40). Factor 6 is clearly a factor of psychoticism (1, 4, 13, and 33, and requires only item 45 to complete the proposed psychoticism scale. Factor 7 is a mixture of three items from anxiety (12, 30, and 41), two from depression (9 and 10), one from interpersonal sensitivity (14) and one from obsessive compulsion (28). Factor 8 includes the five items from the depression scale, (9, 10, 11, 27, and 42), two from the anxiety scale (30 and 41), two from obsessive compulsion (21 and 28), one from psychoticism (45), one from hostility (35) and one from interpersonal sensitivity (14). Finally, factor 9 comprises five items from the obsessive compulsion scale (16, 20, 21, and 28), and would require only item 25 for the scale to be complete. The highest correlations found were between factor 1 and factors 4, 5, 7, and 8, which are those in which items from all of the scales are combined.
To improve the interpretation of the results, the items that had the highest saturation in each factor were selected.
The results were as follows:
Factor 1: 2 (PI), 5 (PI), 19 (PI), 40 (PI), 44 (PI); 14 (IS), 15 (IS), 11 (D), 27 (D)
Factor 2: 3 (PA), 8 (PA), 22 (PA), 24 (PA), 37 (PA), 38 (A), 6 (A)
Factor 3: 18 (S), 23 (S), 26 (S), 29 (S), 31 (S), 25 (OC)
Factor 4: 7 (H), 34 (H), 35 (H), 39 (H), 43 (H)
Factor 5: 17 (IS), 32 (IS), 36 (IS), 42 (D)
Factor 6: 1 (PS), 4 (PS), 13 (PS), 33 (PS) Factor 7: 12 (A), 30 (A), 41 (A)
Factor 8: 9 (D), 10 (D), 28 (OC), 45 (PS)
Factor 9: 16 (OC), 20 (OC), 21 (OC)
In Sandín et al´s. study (2008) the high level of correlation that exists between the items corresponding to the dimensions of paranoid ideation, interpersonal sensitivity and depression seems to indicate a content validity problem since there is a mixture of items in the same factor.
In order to obtain more information regarding the best way to assign the items to the various scales, a group of experts was asked to carry out a qualitative analysis, assigning each item to the proposed scales. The results highlighted the lack of consistency in the classifications, a fault that could be due to the imprecise definition of the different dimensions or of the constructs measured.
Confirmatory factor analysis of the SA-45
Various confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) were carried out, testing several models: (1) the nine-factor model found in the preceding exploratory factor analysis (EFA), (2) the nine-factor model proposed in Sandín et al. (2008), (3) a model with nine first-order factors and a secondorder factor, which could be called ‘emotional maladjustment'. The results are shown in Table 2.
χ2/gl | NNFI | CFI | SRMR | RMSEA | AIC | AGFI | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1: nine basic factors. | 1.16 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.068 | 0.027 | 1417.10 | 0.98 |
Model 2: structure proposed by Sandín et al. | 1.61 | 0.99 | 0.92 | 0.084 | 0.048 | 1865.06 | 0.91 |
Model 3: nine first-order and one second-order factor. | 1.63 | 0.99 | 0.99 | 0.091 | 0.046 | 1813,24 | 0.96 |
Note.NNFI = Non-Normed Fit Index; CFI = Comparative Fit Index; SRMR= Standardized Root Mean Square Residual; RMSEA= Root Mean Square Error of Approximation; AIC = Akaike´s Information Criterion; AGFI= Adjusted Goodness of Fit Index.
As shown, although all three models have good fits, the first seems to be the most suitable according to the global fit indexes values. The global fit model ranged from χ2/gl = 1.16 for model 1 to 1.63 for model 3. The AGFI values ranged from .99 (model 1) to .96 (model 3). The rest of the fit indexes were better for the model 1. This implies that model 1 represents in a better way the relationship between the items of SA-45. That is, model 1 comprised by nine basic factors defined above could be the best representation of the data.
Reliability of the SA-45
The Cronbach's alpha coefficients of both the questionnaire as a whole and each of the nine scales were calculated. Table 3 shows the results obtained from the data for Model 1. Along with the alpha coefficient of each of the scales, an item-test correlation, and the coefficient of the scale if the corresponding item were eliminated, are included. Likewise, the mean is included, as well as the standard deviation of each scale and of the total. The values obtained from Cronbach's alpha coefficient are quite high, with the exception of that for the psychoticism scale.
Item | Correlation Item-Test corrected | Cronbach's Alpha If eliminated |
Mean | SD |
---|---|---|---|---|
Factor 1 Paranoid Ideation (α = 0.87) | 5.24 | 5.34 | ||
2. Others are to blame for most of your troubles | .523 | .863 | ||
5. Most people cannot be trusted | .477 | .868 | ||
11. Feeling no interest in things | .574 | .858 | ||
14. Feeling others do not understand or are unsympathetic | .760 | .839 | ||
15. Feeling that people are unfriendly or dislike you | .653 | .851 | ||
19. Feeling that you are watched or talked about by others | .583 | .857 | ||
27. Feeling hopeless about the future | .572 | .859 | ||
40. Others not giving you proper credit for achievement | .629 | .852 | ||
44. Feeling that people will take advantage of you if you let them | .703 | .845 | ||
Factor 2 Phobic Anxiety (α = 0.84) | 1.40 | 2.67 | ||
3. Feeling afraid in open spaces or in streets | .680 | .799 | ||
6. Suddenly scared for no reason | .448 | .838 | ||
8. Feeling afraid to go out of your house alone | .586 | .814 | ||
22. Feeling afraid to travel on buses, subways, or trains | .677 | .800 | ||
24. Having to avoid certain things, places, or activities because they frighten you | .671 | .799 | ||
37. Feeling uneasy in crowds | .642 | .805 | ||
38. Spells of terror or panic | .525 | .828 | ||
Factor 3 Somatisation (α = 0.80) | 2.73 | 3.15 | ||
18. Soreness of your muscles | .528 | .789 | ||
23. Hot or cold spells | .526 | .770 | ||
25. Your mind going blank | .469 | .780 | ||
26. Numbness or tingling in parts of your body | .648 | .740 | ||
29. Faintness or dizziness | .613 | .747 | ||
31. Feeling everything is an effort | .609 | .752 | ||
Factor 4 Hostility (α = 0.80) | 1.04 | 2.02 | ||
7. Temper outbursts that you could not control | .670 | .730 | ||
34. Having urges to beat, injure or harm someone | .460 | .803 | ||
35. Urges to break or smash things | .611 | .750 | ||
39. Getting into frequent arguments | .595 | .772 | ||
43. Shouting or throwing things | .674 | .734 | ||
Factor 5 Interpersonal Sensitivity (α = 0.82) | 1.69 | 2.58 | ||
17. Feeling inferior to others | .640 | .779 | ||
32. Feeling very self-conscious with others | .605 | .803 | ||
36. Feeling shy and uneasy with the opposite sex | .675 | .762 | ||
42. Feeling of worthlessness | .686 | .762 | ||
Factor 6 Psychoticism (α = 0.57) | 0.48 | 1.13 | ||
1. The idea that someone else can control your thoughts | .346 | .566 | ||
4. Hearing voices that other people do not hear | .369 | .538 | ||
13. Other people being aware of your private thoughts | .436 | .413 | ||
33. Having thoughts that are not your own | .441 | .457 | ||
Factor 7 Anxiety (α = 0.87) | 2.30 | 2.53 | ||
12. Feeling so restless you couldn't sit still | .735 | .846 | ||
30. Worrying too much about things | .779 | .806 | ||
41. Nervousness or shakiness inside | .770 | .818 | ||
Factor 8 Depression (α = 0.75) | 2.32 | 2.53 | ||
9. Feeling lonely | .643 | .641 | ||
10. Feeling blue | .682 | .619 | ||
28. Trouble concentrating | .572 | .688 | ||
45. The idea that you should be punished for your sins | .383 | .783 | ||
Factor 9 Obsessive Compulsion (α = 0.77) | 1.35 | 1.89 | ||
16. Having to do things very slowly to ensure correctness | .631 | .655 | ||
20. Having to check and double check what you do | .636 | .671 | ||
21. Difficulty making decisions | .567 | .748 | ||
Scale total α = 0.95 | 18.53 | 18.35 |
This suggests that the assignment of the items to mension has been adequate and appropriate. In table 4, the correlation between factors is presented.
Scale | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Psychological D. | --- | ||||||||
2. Phobic anxiety | .443 | --- | |||||||
3. Somatisation | .504 | .467 | --- | ||||||
4. Hostility | .611 | .405 | .431 | --- | |||||
5. Interpersonal sensitivity | .711 | .487 | .452 | .497 | --- | ||||
6. Psychoticism | .275 | .256 | .178 | .345 | .332 | --- | |||
7. Anxiety | .579 | .375 | .537 | .601 | .502 | .138 | --- | ||
8. Depression | .585 | .427 | .486 | .626 | .518 | .073 | .557 | --- | |
9. Obsessive compulsion | .444 | .400 | .469 | .452 | .395 | .275 | .356 | .528 | --- |
Discussion and Conclusions
One point that the results have highlighted is that the best solution found has nuances that differentiate it from the solutions proposed by other authors (Alvarado et al., 2012; Sandín et al., 2008) who have replicated Maruish et al.'s (1998) original model.
We understand that using a non-clinical population in the initial studies as opposed to a clinical one as in Maruish et al.'s study (1998) will in large part determine the results that are obtained. One of the first points that stand out is that the majority of the responses fall into the 0 and 1 categories. For example, for the item “Hear voices that other people do not hear”, 97.8 % fell into category 0. This is not an isolated case; for as many as 7 items, over 90 % of the responses are concentrated in the 0 category. If we consider the first two response categories, 30 of the 45 items show over 90 % of responses falling into these two categories. This fact alone may show that the composition of these items is not appropriate for a “normal” population, or in this case, for participants without diagnosed psychopathological disorders.
In this study, we have not replicated the solution proposed by Maruish et al. (1998), though other authors have in fact done so (Alvarado et al., 2012; Sandín et al., 2008).
It is worth highlighting that in contrast to the previous studies, we discovered a factor in the EFA that explains one third of the total variability, which leads us to consider the existence of a factor of general character. This general character must have great specific weight in the configuration of any explanatory model on the dimensionality of the instrument.
Nevertheless, the intention in carrying out a CFA was to test Maruish et al's (1998) original model (model 2); this model has also been replicated by Sandín et al. (2008).
Specifically, we have discovered that the global fit indexes were acceptable for our data. Nonetheless, given that the sample used by Maruish et al. (1998) was of clinical subjects, that the response pattern of our study may be atypical for a non-clinical population, and that the results obtained in the EFA may suggest alternative factorial solutions, another two models were tested.
The first model, consistent with the results of the EFA, comprised nine dimensions, of which the majority contained a combination of items from Maruish et al.'s original dimensions. The first dimension, and the most explanatory, comprised a total of nine elements from three factors (paranoid ideation, interpersonal sensitivity and depression); the second factor had seven items related to phobic anxiety and anxiety. The global fit indexes of this solution are clearly better than those of the original model.
On the other hand, exploring the possibility that the model was one-dimensional, a third model was tested (model 3), with nine first-order factors and a general second-order factor. The fit indexes were also acceptable, but although they are better than those obtained for the original model, they are worse than those obtained for model 1.
We understand that if the SA-45 is applied to a nonclinical population, we must evaluate whether scores ought to be corrected by Maruish et al.'s solution due to reasonable doubts as to whether the questionnaire is the best option for a non-clinical population. In this sense, we understand that the solution presented is best used as a measurement of the general mental health of the subjects rather than as a purely diagnostic test to be used in a psychiatric context.
Factor 1 encompasses elements of paranoid ideation, interpersonal sensitivity and depression. All the elements of factor 2 have the word “fear” in common, which may lead to the belief that its elements are not understood in the same way as in a population of clinical subjects. In factor 3, there is a mixture of items from the categories of somatisation and obsessive compulsion. The fourth factor makes clear reference to hostility, while in the fifth, elements of depression and interpersonal sensitivity are combined again. In other words, a subject who is probably not depressed will not interpret the combined content of the items in the same way clinical subjects interpret them. Factor 6 is, again, interpreted as psychoticism. Factor 7 is saturated with only three items from the original scale of anxiety that make reference to tension or nervousness. The eighth dimension includes a mixture from the categories of depression, obsessive compulsion and psychoticism. Specifically, the items refer to feeling sad, alone, incapable of concentrating and having constant thoughts of selfpunishment. Finally, factor 9 refers to obsessive compulsion, and the items that make up this dimension are related to being indecisive and insecure.
The discrepant results obtained in this study suggest that there is a need to continue researching the factorial structure of the SA-45 in the non-clinical population. It could be concluded that the items are not understood in the same way when applied to this sample as when they are applied to a clinical sample group. In fact, different dimensions arise from those proposed by Maruish et al. (1998) in the studies carried out with non-clinical population that may be related to personality traits. For this reason, more empirical evidence must be obtained on the configuration and the significance of the constructs in the SA-45 for the non-clinical population. Furthermore, the items should be adapted to this type of the population as necessary.