SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.45 número1El papel del mindfulness rasgo en la calidad de vida de la población adulta: una revisión sistemáticaRevisión sistemática sobre el funcionamiento familiar en familias con un hijo con una enfermedad poco frecuente índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Revista

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • En proceso de indezaciónCitado por Google
  • No hay articulos similaresSimilares en SciELO
  • En proceso de indezaciónSimilares en Google

Compartir


Papeles del Psicólogo

versión On-line ISSN 1886-1415versión impresa ISSN 0214-7823

Pap. Psicol. vol.45 no.1 Madrid ene./abr. 2024  Epub 29-Ene-2024

https://dx.doi.org/10.23923/pap.psicol.3030 

ARTICLES

Memory wars: a solution to the conflict

Las guerras de la memoria: una solución al conflicto

Antonio L Manzanero (orcid: 0000-0003-2619-4571)1  , Claudia Morales-Valiente (orcid: 0000-0002-2181-5087)2 

1Complutense University of Madrid (Spain)

2University of Western Ontario (Canada)

Abstract

The validity of dissociative memory in forensic contexts and the causes of the so-called "memory wars" are discussed. Misconceptions between clinical and forensic psychology, a deficient definition of amnesia, and the difficulties inherent in studying traumatic memories contribute to the persistence of this controversy. Particularly in the field of forensic psychology, the debate could be attributed to the lack of consensus on scientific evidence. Psychologists need to establish an empirical foundation to understand better the mechanisms of memory involved in remembering and forgetting traumatic memories. The Continuous Accessibility Model of Memory is outlined to explain the retrieval of the different degrees of accessibility to autobiographical memories based on different factors.

Keywords: Traumatic memory; Amnesia; Victims; Forensic psychology

Resumen

Se discute la validez de la memoria disociativa en contextos forenses y las causas de las llamadas "guerras de la memoria". Conceptos erróneos entre la psicología clínica y la psicología forense, una definición deficiente de la amnesia y las dificultades inherentes en el estudio de recuerdos traumáticos contribuyen a la persistencia de esta controversia. Particularmente en el campo de la psicología forense, el debate podría atribuirse a la falta de consenso sobre la evidencia científica. Los psicólogos necesitan establecer una base empírica para comprender mejor los mecanismos de la memoria involucrados en recordar y olvidar recuerdos traumáticos. Se esboza el Modelo de Accesibilidad Continua de la Memoria para explicar la recuperación de los diferentes grados de accesibilidad a los recuerdos autobiográficos basados en diferentes factores.

Palabras clave: Memoria traumática; Amnesia; Víctimas; Psicología forense

Introduction

The so-called "memory wars" controversy (Loftus, 2004) surrounding dissociative amnesia within forensic contexts has been closed for some time (Freyd et al., 2010; Lindblom & Gray, 2010; Loftus, 1993; Loftus & Ketcham, 1996). However, it has recently resurfaced and reopened an interesting discussion (Brand et al., 2017a, 2017b, 2018; Patihis et al., 2021; Merckelbach & Patihis, 2018; Patihis et al., 2019) with special impact on forensic contexts.

The acceptance of repressed and later recovered memories phenomenon (i.e., dissociative amnesia or repressed memories) can lead to judicial errors in the form of bad practices when attempting to elicit memories of events that had not taken place (Arce et al., 2023; Loftus, 2004). Accepting dissociative amnesia should not be limited to whether the courts accept this phenomenon (Patihis et al., 2019), as courts are not the space for psychological theorizing, and theoretical ambiguities are inadmissible. In fact, the phenomenon of repressed memories or dissociative amnesias is not currently admissible in court as it fails to meet the criteria established in Daubert vs. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc. (1993).

The persistence of false dissociative amnesias can be attributed to several factors. Firstly, the confusion between clinical psychology and forensic psychology leads to a misunderstanding of the diagnosis and treatment of dissociative amnesia. Secondly, the inadequate definition of amnesia obscures the understanding of the phenomenon. Lastly, inherent difficulties in researching traumatic memories pose challenges to understanding the true nature of dissociative amnesia. The discord and misinformation generated among professionals due to the lack of consensus on the scientific evidence is worrisome.

This article proposes the Continuous Memory Accessibility Model. The model represents the variabilities in access to autobiographical memories depending on the causes of different memory conditions. This model poses an alternative to resolve the discussions around dissociative amnesias in the forensic context.

Clinical vs. Forensic Psychology

Clinical and forensic psychology are distinct fields with different objectives, methods, and requirements (Greenberg & Shuman, 1997). While dissociative amnesia may be relevant in the psychotherapeutic context, it cannot be accepted uncritically in forensic psychology. In forensic psychology, scientific support and theoretical agreement are particularly relevant as the underlying reality of the alleged event is crucial.

One of the main reasons the assumption of dissociative amnesia persists is its acceptance in a therapeutic context. Most memory experts deem the repression of traumatic memories implausible and the possibility of remembering them in a therapeutic context. Despite this, many professionals, students, and the general public continue to hold onto this belief (Patihis et al., 2021).

In the psychotherapeutic space, patients may, for example, report that they cannot leave the house due to a past attack or use the excuse of not remembering traumatic events to avoid talking about them. In this case, the therapist's main concern is the patient's subjective experience and mental health in the present moment, beyond the event's occurrence and the memory's accuracy. When a patient reports an inability to remember certain events, the therapist does not necessarily question the accuracy of the patient's memory unless it assists the therapeutic process. By contrast, in forensic contexts, the primary goal is to determine the factual accuracy of the person's statements, and, therefore, the truthfulness of recollection gains significance and requires more rigorous assessment.

From a survival perspective, it is not helpful to forget traumatic experiences, even though they may be painful. When working with victims of natural disasters or wars, it is common to find cases where people claim not to remember their traumatic experiences. However, research suggests that the real problem is that they want to forget but cannot (Manzanero et al., 2020). In a study of memories of sexual assault, Porter and Birt (2001) found that traumatic memories tend to be recalled more frequently than other autobiographical memories. Human memory processes emotionally significant information and events distinctively, leading to traumatic memories taking priority over others. In cases where these events have been forgotten, it is more likely due to a deliberate attempt to suppress the memory than to repressed or dissociated amnesia. Memories of traumatic events are likelier to create a flashbulb memory (Brown & Kulik, 1977) rather than amnesia (Hirst et al., 2015).

Amnesia vs. Poor Memory

Another element that generates debate in dissociative amnesia is the poor definition of what is meant by amnesia. Amnesia, which implies a total lack of memory, is confused with bad memory, but remembering poorly is not the same as remembering nothing. The DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) contributes to the confusion when, in some pathologies such as PTSD, it mentions the "inability to recall key features of the trauma" as a criterion (Manzanero et al., 2020).

The reduction of cognitive resources by the effect of high levels of activation (stress) (Yerkes & Dodson, 1908) produces a narrowing of the attentional focus (Easterbrook, 1959). It hinders the integration processes in the generation of memory traces. As a result, the memories of traumatic events associated with high anxiety levels are characterized by a detailed description of the central information but with few peripheral details (Byrne et al., 2001). Traumatic memories frequently appear fragmented and predominantly sensorial (as processing sensory information requires few cognitive resources). This lack of cognitive resources in the coding phase would also cause weak memories, hindering in-depth information processing and the establishment of associations with prior knowledge and experience (Craik & Lockhart, 1972).

In traumatic events, deficits in encoding processes would hinder explicit (controlled) retrieval but allow implicit (automatic) retrieval, likely leading to incidental memories instead of deliberate ones (Graf & Mandler, 1984; Schacter, 1987). Incidental retrieval involves non-conscious retrieval processes; thus, it is not possible to identify the recovery signs that make this memory accessible, and therefore, it is not possible to control them (Baddeley, 1990).

Depersonalization is often interpreted as a symptom of amnesic dissociation in victims of traumatic events. However, it is normal for autobiographical memories to be recovered from different points of view, such as an observer's perspective (in the third person or as if it were a film) or from the protagonist's perspective. This difference is not necessarily due to a pathology like dissociation, which is serious and rare, but rather to the normal functioning of memory (D'Argembeau et al., 2003; Manzanero et al., 2015). It is also an effect of multiple recoveries that is typical in traumatic memories, and this can also be seen in the earliest and most elaborated memories (Crawley & French, 2005).

The current misconceptions about dissociative amnesia, viewing it as an amnesia rather than a phenomenon resulting from the normal functioning of memory in extreme psychological situations, contribute to the confusion surrounding the nature of dissociative amnesia. In reality, dissociative amnesia involves deficient coding of the event and the generation of incomplete or deficient memory traces that complicate its posterior recovery. Additionally, the complex verification of traumatic mechanisms, low incidence phenomenon, and important ethical implications in investigations all contribute to the maintenance of the phenomenon.

Problems in Studying Traumatic Memories

Brewin (2007) points out controversy due to variability in the findings on traumatic memories, with the sample being quoted as the main cause. Firstly, there is a problem with the presence of pathologies: studies involving clinical samples may not accurately represent the general population (Steel et al., 2009; Dimitry, 2012; Manzanero et al., 2021). Secondly, there is the problem of unverifiability of the event in non-clinical samples: it can be difficult to accurately assess the veracity of traumatic experiences, such as childhood traumatic events. Finally, there is the problem of the uncontrolled samples: studying victims of wars and catastrophes provides more reliable information on the characteristics of traumatic memories since the reality of the experience can be easily established (Patihis et al., 2019). However, extraneous variables such as organic damage, drug effects, pharmacological treatments, sleep deficits, and nutritional deficiencies affect the study result.

Many countries have proposed that sexual crimes against minors be considered imprescriptible, and associations of victims of child sexual abuse have arisen to uncover these types of crimes that have remained hidden for decades. However, proving such crimes is extremely difficult and some have advocated for the victim's testimony to be sufficient to establish a conviction, even if it distorts the presumption of innocence (Subijana & Echeburúa, 2018). In this context, memory experts have raised concerns about certain iatrogenic psychotherapeutic practices, such as guided imagination, dream interpretation, hypnosis, sodium amytal administration, and bibliotherapy, which may induce false memories in patients (Loftus, 2004). These false memories, combined with erroneous indicators of abuse, could lead to false complaints, even if the intention was to detect "repressed" cases of sexual abuse in childhood.

Factors such as the age of the victim at the time of the event(s), whether it was a single occurrence or repeated over time, the duration and type of aggression, the dynamics of the recall process, and the procedures followed to obtain the victim's statement; should be considered to minimize false accusations based on false memories. It is also important to consider whether it could be a "repressed" and then recovered memory, the number of victims, the relationship between the victim and the perpetrator, and any other relevant factors. For instance, it is not the same to consider a case of sexual assault that lasted for years during childhood and was always remembered by the victim but not reported due to embarrassment or fear, compared to a case where an adult victim undergoes therapeutic procedures to remember a single instance of sexual assault when they were less than five years old by a family member. While both cases may involve sexual assault, the latter is more likely to be a false memory than a recovered one.

The following is a model proposed to facilitate understanding the different degrees of accessibility of memories based on different factors.

Continuous Accessibility Model of Memory

Forensic psychologists have struggled to provide conclusive and reliable evidence for the existence of dissociative amnesia of traumatic events. Accumulated evidence alludes to a more vivid and repeated recall of traumatic events (Hirst et al., 2015; Manzanero et al., 2020; Porter & Birt, 2001). Nevertheless, reaching a consensus is important to conclude whether dissociative amnesia could or could not be considered in court.

Psychologists need to establish a solid empirical foundation to understand better the mechanisms of memory involved in remembering and forgetting traumatic memories. As suggested by Patihis et al. (2021), it is crucial to explore the nature of traumatic memories, their relationship with dissociation, and the phenomenon of repressed memories. A particular focus should be placed on studying the psychological mechanisms involved in motivated forgetting, retrieval inhibition (Catarino et al., 2015), and its association with emotion (Gagnepain et al., 2017).

Empirical evidence has challenged theoretical perspectives on memory deficits in trauma victims, as the accessibility of traumatic memories can vary greatly (see Figure 1). While amnesia is typically associated with organic damage resulting from head injuries, encoding conditions and retention factors can result in poorly detailed memories that are still considered memories, even without organic pathology. However, most traumatic experiences generate memories with expected characteristics and accessibility, particularly in primary and indirect victims. Social and individual factors can also contribute to memories with greater accessibility and vividness, which may be a precursor to PTSD, a memory disorder characterized by chronic accessibility of traumatic memories in the form of flashbacks, ruminations, or nightmares, with increasingly intense emotions associated with each recall (Rubin et al., 2008).

Figure 1. Continuous Accessibility Model of Memory, According to the Causes That Generate the Different Types of Memory 

Individuals with PTSD tend to recall traumatic events in greater detail, and this is related to the number of recalls they make, which may lead to an over-generalization of retrieval cues (Desmedt, 2021). This over-generalization could reinforce the connections in the brain and increase memory accessibility, which is a step closer to developing PTSD.

However, some studies indicate that the risk of PTSD among victims of wars decreases significantly over time (Ehlers & Clark, 2003; Giacco & Priebe, 2018; Ssenyonga et al., 2013). These findings suggest that even for traumatic events, there is a natural decline in memory vividness, and coping with trauma may be promoted through time and acceptance of greater variability in recall, with less detail and more distance and perspective.

In the forensic context, it is important to distinguish the typical expression of the cause behind the memory impairment. For example, traumatic events likely lead to high accessibility of memories, although, as we noted above, these memories often appear fragmented and predominantly sensory. In the same sense, the model makes an important distinction between amnesia and poor memory because, as we said before, remembering poorly is not the same as not remembering anything. Finally, the model considers populations physically affected, healthy and exposed to traumatic events in a single continuum, allowing its use in different contexts.

Conclusions

This paper discusses the controversy surrounding dissociative amnesia in forensic contexts. The confusion between clinical psychology and forensic psychology, an inadequate definition of amnesia, and methodological problems in studying traumatic memories are core issues in this discussion. It is important to note that dissociative amnesia may be relevant in the psychotherapeutic context. However, it cannot be uncritically accepted in forensic psychology, where the underlying reality of the alleged event is the main point.

The Continuous Accessibility Model of Memory poses an alternative for understanding the different degrees of accessibility to memories depending on the causes. While organic causes are mainly associated with amnesia and/or poor memory, psychological and physical causes are associated with flashbulb memories and PTSD. Moreover, memory accessibility moves in a continuum from low accessibility, where the accuracy of recall is affected, to high accessibility, where control of recall is affected. The model also refers to the frequency on which these levels of accessibility appear in the general population in the form of a bell curve, with standard memory representing the “healthy” level of accessibility. However, it is important to point out that apparent “quality of recall” is not an indicator of veracity in testimony. The fact that a potential victim cannot remember the events does not necessarily mean they did not occur, as can happen in cases of crimes committed through chemical submission (Quintana et al., 2020). Similarly, remembering them is not indicative that it did happen, as demonstrated in cases of false autobiographical memories (Arce et al., 2023).

Acknowledgements

This paper is part of the research project financed by Santander-Universidad Complutense de Madrid (PR87/19-22576).

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). [ Links ]

Arce, R., Selaya, A., Sanmarco, J., & Fariña, F. (2023). Implanting rich autobiographical false memories: Meta-analysis for forensic practice and judicial judgment making. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 23(4), 100386. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijchp.2023.100386 [ Links ]

Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Human memory. Theory and practice. LEA. [ Links ]

Brand, B. L., Dalenberg, C. J., Frewen, P. A., Loewenstein, P. J., Schielke, H. J., Brams, J. S., & Spiegel, D. (2018). Trauma-related dissociation is no fantasy: Addressing the errors of omissions and errors commission in Merckelbach and Patihis (2018). Psychological Injury & Law, 11, 377-393. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12207-018-9336-8 [ Links ]

Brand, B. L., Schielke, H. J., & Brams, J. S. (2017a). Assisting the courts in understanding and connecting with experiences of disconnection: Addressing trauma-related dissociation as a forensic psychologist, part I. Psychological Injury and Law, 10, 283-297. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12207-017-9304-8 [ Links ]

Brand, B. L., Schielke, H. J., Brams, J. S., & DiComo, R. A. (2017b). Assessing trauma-related dissociation in forensic contexts: Addressing trauma-related dissociation as a forensic psychologist, part II. Psychological Injury and Law, 10, 298-312. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12207-017-9305-7 [ Links ]

Brewin, C. R. (2007). Autobiographical memory for trauma: Update on four controversies. Memory, 15, 227-248. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210701256423 [ Links ]

Brown, R., & Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition, 5, 73-99. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(77)90018-X [ Links ]

Byrne, C. A., Hyman, I. E., & Scott, K. L. (2001). Comparisons of memories for traumatic events and other experiences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 119-133. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.837 [ Links ]

Catarino, A., Küpper, C. S., Werner-Seidler, A., Dalgleish, T., & Anderson, M. C. (2015). Failing to forget: Inhibitory-control deficits compromise memory suppression in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Science, 26(5), 604-616. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615569889 [ Links ]

Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A frame work for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671-684. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80001-X [ Links ]

Crawley, S., & French, C. (2005). Field and observer viewpoint in remember-know memories of personal childhood events. Memory, 13(7), 673-681. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210444000296 [ Links ]

D'Argembeau, A., Comblain, C., & Linden, M. van der (2003). Phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories for positive, negative, and neutral events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17(3), 281-294. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.856 [ Links ]

Daubert vs. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc. (1993). 113 S. Ct. 2786. [ Links ]

Desmedt, A. (2021). (Re) contextualizing the trauma to prevent or treat PTSD-related hypermnesia. Chronic Stress, 5: 24705470211021073. https://doi.org/10.1177/24705470211021073 [ Links ]

Dimitry, L. (2012). A systematic review on the mental health of children and adolescents in areas of armed conflict in the Middle East. Child: Care, Health and Development, 38(2), 153-161. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2214.2011.01246.x [ Links ]

Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on the utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183-201. https://.doi.org/10.1037/h0047707 [ Links ]

Ehlers, A., & Clark, D. (2003). Early psychological interventions for adult survivors of trauma: A review. Biological Psychiatry, 53(9), 817-826. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01812-7 [ Links ]

Freyd, J. J., Klest, B., & DePrince, A. P. (2010). Avoiding awareness of betrayal: Comment on Lindblom and Gray (2009). Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(1), 20-26. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1555 [ Links ]

Gagnepain, P., Hulbert, J., & Anderson, M. C. (2017). Parallel regulation of memory and emotion supports the suppression of intrusive memories. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(27), 6423-6441. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2732-16.2017 [ Links ]

Giacco, D., & Priebe, S. (2018). Mental health care for adult refugees in high-income countries. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 27(2), 109-116. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796017000609 [ Links ]

Graf, P., & Mandler, G. (1984). Activation makes words more accessible, but not necessarily more retrievable. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 553-568. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(84)90346-3 [ Links ]

Greenberg, S. A., & Shuman, D. W. (1997). Irreconcilable conflict between therapeutic and forensic roles. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 28(1), 50-57. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.28.1.50 [ Links ]

Hirst, W., Phelps, E. A., Meksin, R., Vaidya, C. J., Johnson, M. K., Mitchell, K. J., Buckner, R. L., Budson, A. E., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Lustig, C., Mather, M., Ochsner, K. N., Schacter, D., Simons, J. S., Lyle, K. B., Cuc, A. F., & Olsson, A. (2015). A ten-year follow-up of a study of memory for the attack of September 11, 2001: Flashbulb memories and memories for flashbulb events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(3), 604-623. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000055 [ Links ]

Lindblom, K. M. & Gray, M. J. (2010). Relationship closeness and trauma narrative detail: A critical analysis of betrayal trauma theory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(1), 1-19. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1547 [ Links ]

Loftus, E. F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist, 48(5), 518-537. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.48.5.518 [ Links ]

Loftus, E. F. (2004). Dispatch from the (un)civil memory wars. The Lancet, 362, 20-21. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(04)17626-5 [ Links ]

Loftus, E., & Ketcham, K. (1996). The myth of repressed memory: False memories and allegations of sexual abuse. Macmillan. [ Links ]

Manzanero, A. L., Crespo, M., Barón, S., Scott, M. T., El-Astal, S., & Hemaid, F. (2021). Traumatic events exposure and psychological trauma in children victims of war in the Gaza Strip. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 36(3-4), 1568-1587. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260517742911 [ Links ]

Manzanero, A. L., Fernández, J., Gómez-Gutiérrez, M. M., Álvarez, M. A., El-Astal, S., Hemaid, F., & Veronese, G. (2020). Between happiness and sorrow: Phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories concerning war episodes and positive events in the Gaza Strip. Memory Studies, 13(6), 917-931. https://doi.org/10.1177/1750698018818221 [ Links ]

Manzanero, A. L., López, B., Aróztegui, J., & El-Astal, S. (2015). Autobiographical memories for negative and positive events in war contexts. Anuario de Psicología Jurídica, 25, 57-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apj.2015.02.001 [ Links ]

Merckelbach, H., & Patihis, L. (2018). Why “trauma-related dissociation” is a misnomer in courts: A critical analysis of Brand et al. (2017a, b). Psychological Injury and Law, 11, 370-376. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12207-018-9328-8 [ Links ]

Patihis, L., Ho, L. Y., Loftus, E. F., & Herrera, M. E. (2021). Memory experts’ beliefs about repressed memory. Memory, 29(6), 823-828. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2018.1532521 [ Links ]

Patihis, L., Otgaar, H., & Merckelbach, H. (2019). Expert Witnesses, Dissociative Amnesia, and Extraordinary Remembering: Response to Brand et al. Psychological Injury and Law, 12, 281-285. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12207-019-09348-8 [ Links ]

Porter, S., & Birt, A. R. (2001). Is traumatic memory special? A comparison of traumatic memory characteristics with memory for other emotional life experiences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 101-117. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.766 [ Links ]

Quintana, J. M., García-Maroto, A., Moreno, O., & Manzanero, A. L. (2020). Characteristics of drug-facilitated sexual assaults in Spain. Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling, 17(3), 215-223. https://doi.org/10.1002/jip.1550 [ Links ]

Rubin, D. C., Berntsen, D., & Bohni, M. K. (2008). A memory-based model of posttraumatic stress disorder: evaluating basic assumptions underlying the PTSD diagnosis. Psychological Review, 115(4), 985-1011. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013397 [ Links ]

Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit expressions of memory in organic amnesia: learning of never facts and associations. Human Neurobiology, 6, 107-118. [ Links ]

Ssenyonga, J., Owens, V., & Olema, D. K. (2013). Posttraumatic growth, resilience, and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) among refugees. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 82, 144-148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.06.238 [ Links ]

Steel, Z., Chey, T., Silove, D., Marnane, C., Bryant, R. A., & Ommeren, M. van (2009). Association of torture and other potentially traumatic events with mental health outcomes among populations exposed to mass conflict and displacement: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Jama, 302(5), 537-549. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2009.1132 [ Links ]

Subijana, I. J., & Echeburúa, E. (2018). Los menores víctimas de abuso sexual en el proceso judicial: el control de la victimización secundaria y las garantías jurídicas de los acusados. [Child sexual abuse in the criminal justice system: Preventing secondary victimization in children and providing legal protection for accused persons]. Anuario de Psicología Jurídica, 28, 22-27. https://doi.org/10.5093/apj2018a1 [ Links ]

Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. Journal of Comparative and Neurological Psychology, 18, 459-482. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.920180503 [ Links ]

Received: June 05, 2023; Accepted: September 30, 2023

Correspondence: antonio.manzanero@psi.ucm.es

The authors reported no potential conflict of interest.

Creative Commons License This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License