SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.38 número1El encubrimiento y las teorías de conspiración: datos demográficos, decepción en el trabajo, sensibilidad a la equidad y creencias en el encubrimientoGanar y recuperar atractivo organizacional durante el proceso de reclutamiento: un estudio factorial multisegmentario con viñetas índice de autoresíndice de assuntospesquisa de artigos
Home Pagelista alfabética de periódicos  

Serviços Personalizados

Journal

Artigo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • Em processo de indexaçãoCitado por Google
  • Não possue artigos similaresSimilares em SciELO
  • Em processo de indexaçãoSimilares em Google

Compartilhar


Revista de Psicología del Trabajo y de las Organizaciones

versão On-line ISSN 2174-0534versão impressa ISSN 1576-5962

Rev. psicol. trab. organ. vol.38 no.1 Madrid Abr. 2022  Epub 04-Abr-2022

https://dx.doi.org/10.5093/jwop2022a3 

ARTICLES

Cross-cultural evidence of the relationship between subjective well-being and job performance: a meta-analysis

Evidencia transcultural de la relación entre el bienestar subjetivo y el desempeño en el trabajo: un metaanálisis

Jesús F Salgado1  , Silvia Moscoso1 

1University of Santiago de Compostela, Spain

ABSTRACT

This meta-analysis examined the differences across countries/regions, and the moderator effects of the study type (cross-sectional vs. longitudinal) on the SWB-job performance relationship. The database consists of 78 independent samples (N = 18,853), located through electronic and manual searches. The results showed that overall SWB (ρ = .37), cognitive SWB (ρ = .27), and affective SWB (ρ = .37) are predictors of job performance. Evidence of cross-cultural effects showed that the magnitude of the SWB-job performance relationship was larger in the Asia-Pacific region than in Europe and the US-Canada region (Asia-Pacific ρ = .41, Europe ρ = .33, USA ρ = .23). Moderator analyses indicated that, on average, cross-sectional (concurrent) and longitudinal (predictive) studies showed similar validity (ρ = .33 vs. ρ = .32). Lastly, we discuss the main contributions, and some practical implications and some limitations of the study are mentioned.

Keywords Subjective well-being; Job performance; Cross-cultural differences; Satisfaction with life; Affective well-being; Cognitive well-bein

RESUMEN

Este metaanálisis examinó las diferencias entre países/regiones y los efectos moderadores del tipo de estudio (transversal vs. longitudinal) en la relación entre bienestar subjetivo (SWB) y desempeño en el trabajo. La base de datos consta de 78 muestras independientes (N = 18,853), localizadas mediante búsquedas electrónicas y manuales. Los resultados mostraron que el SWB global (ρ = .37), el SWB cognitivo (ρ = .27) y el SWB afectivo (ρ = .37) son predictores del desempeño laboral. La evidencia de los efectos transculturales mostró que la magnitud de la relación entre el BS y el desempeño laboral era mayor en la región de Asia-Pacífico que en Europa y en la región de Estados Unidos-Canadá (Asia-Pacífico ρ = .41, Europa ρ = .33, Estados Unidos ρ = .23). Los análisis de moderadores indicaron que, en promedio, los estudios transversales (concurrentes) y los longitudinales (predictivos) mostraron una validez semejante (ρ = .33 frente a ρ = .32). Por último, se discuten las principales contribuciones y se indican algunas implicaciones prácticas y las limitaciones del estudio.

Palabras clave Bienestar subjetivo; Desempeño en el puesto; Diferencias transculturales; Satisfacción con la vida; Bienestar afectivo; Bienestar cognitivo

Introduction

Subjective well-being (SWB) has been the focus of a scientific research corpus comprising thousands of studies all across the world, and which has been – and continues to be – of interest to psychologists, economists, political scientists, neuroscientists, and philosophers. The relationships between SWB and significant life events are currently well established. For example, SWB has correlated with better health and longevity, lower alcohol consumption, higher levels of exercise and not smoking, as well as a lower probability of getting divorced and high-quality relationships with family, friends, and romantic partners (see Adler & Seligman, 2016; Boehm & Kubzanky, 2012; Diener et al., 2018; Diener & Seligman, 2002, 2004; Guven, 2012; Moore & Diener, 2019, for instance). From demographic and societal points of view, SWB has correlated with gender (men scoring higher in SWB than women), age (older people being shown to be happier than young people), gross domestic product (GDP), income, and political freedom (Diener et al., 2018).

In the organizational domain, which is a focus of this article, SWB has been related to a series of critical organizational outcomes. For example, it has been found that happy workers are more likely to be more productive, to display less absenteeism, more creativity, less turnover in employment, to have better relationships with co-workers, users, and supervisors, a higher salary, lower rates of unemployment, higher job satisfaction, higher employee engagement, less sick time, higher firm earnings, financial performance, and successful relocation (see, Boehm & Lyubomirsky, 2008; Bowling et al., 2010; Diener et al., 2020; Erdogan et al., 2012; Ford et al., 2011; Harter et al., 2020; Harter et al., 2010; Harter et al., 2002; Judge et al., 2005; Lyubomirsky et al., 2005; Pujol-Cols & Lazzaro-Salazar, 2021; Sagi et al., 2021; Shockley et al., 2012; Tait et al., 1989; Taris & Schreurs, 2009; Tenney et al., 2016; Thoresen et al., 2003; Walsh et al., 2018).

Recently, Moscoso and Salgado (2021) carried out the largest meta-analysis on the relationship between SWB and job performance ratings conducted until now. They examined the relationship of job performance with overall SWB, cognitive SWB, and affective SWB. They examined the moderator effects of the source of job performance (supervisor ratings vs. self-report) and, based on an explanatory model proposed by Salgado et al. (2019), they also tested a model of a suppressor relationship between cognitive and affective SWB to predict job performance. The main findings of Moscoso and Salgado (2021) were that overall SWB correlated similarly with supervisory ratings and self-reported performance (ρ = -35 and ρ = .33, respectively), although the affective component of SWB correlated higher with supervisory ratings than with self-reported performance (ρ = .49 and ρ = .30, respectively). Finally, they found support for the suppressor effect of cognitive SWB for predicting supervisory ratings.

Moscoso and Salgado’s (2021) meta-analysis did not examine the potential moderator effects of cross-cultural differences. However, international research has also shown that there are differences in the level of SWB across countries, in the way in which individuals frame their well-being experiences, and in how life circumstances affect SWB (see, Diener et al., 2010; Veenhoven, 2018, 2019; Walsh et al., 2018). Therefore, national and cross-cultural differences may affect the SWB-job performance relationship. Walsh et al. (2018) pointed out that the SWB literature oversampled western, predominantly English-speaking populations, with few reviewed studies conducted in Asian samples and none in African and Central and South American samples. According to Walsh et al. (2018), it is very likely that culture moderates the relationship between SWB and organizational success in a variety of still unknown ways. They pointed out that research still needs to focus on a wider range of cultures so that the findings can be generalizable across the world. However, there is not currently a meta-analysis that clarifies if the relationship between SWB and job performance is similar across countries/cultures and if the variability of the effect sizes found in the primary studies mentioned above can be attributed to national and cultural characteristics. This last issue is the main focus of this research: to examine the cross-cultural (international) relationship between SWB and job performance. At present, it is not conclusively known if the SWB-job performance relationships found in Western societies are replicated in other societies/cultures (Boehm & Lyubomirsky, 2008; Walsh et al., 2018).

Subjective Well-being

In this study, we adopt Diener’s (Diener, 1984, 2000; Diener et al., 2003) conceptualization of SWB, according to which SWB refers to the cognitive evaluation and emotional balance that people make of their lives. In Diener’s approach, SWB is conceptualized as a construct consisting of two major components: a cognitive component referring to the judgments of life satisfaction and an emotional component referring to the balance between levels of positive and negative feelings experienced by the individual (Diener & Biswas-Diener, 2008; Diener et al., 2009). Diener’s conception of SWB consists, therefore, of three elements: life satisfaction, positive affect (PA), and negative affect (NA). For this reason, it is sometimes known as the tripartite model of SWB (Busseri, 2018; Tov, 2018).

Overall, high SWB is, therefore, a combination of two specific factors: (1) the presence of positive affective states and the absence of negative affective states, and (2) global life satisfaction. In Diener’s research tradition, satisfaction with life is usually conceptualized as the judgmental component of SWB (Busseri, 2015, 2018; Busseri & Sadava, 2011; Diener, 2000; Diener & Biswas-Diener, 2008; Diener et al. 1985; Fisher, 2010).

Typically, cognitive SWB has been assessed using different scales such as Cantril’s (1965) Self-Anchoring Striving, Diener et al.’s (1985) Satisfaction with Life scale (SWLS), Lyubomirsky and Lepper’s (1999) Happiness scale, Fordyce’s (1988) Global Happiness scale, Tellegen’s (1982) Well-being scale, and Hills and Argyle’s (2002) Oxford Happiness Questionnaire. Probably the most popular of these scales are Cantril’s Ladder and the Satisfaction with Life scale, and for this reason, cognitive SWB has also been referred to many times as satisfaction with life. This component is a judgment on or perception of different aspects of an individual’s life as a whole (Larsen, 2009).

Affective SWB is conceived as the balance between the experience of positive and negative feelings as a reaction to situational conditions. Typically, affective SWB has been evaluated, for instance, with the SPANE scale (Diener & Biswas-Diener, 2008; Diener et al., 2009), the PANAS state scale (Watson et al., 1988), the Profile of Mood States (Heuchert & McNair, 2012), and the Index of Psychological Well-being scale (Berkman, 1971).

Cross-cultural Evidence of Differences in SWB and Job Performance Evaluations

Empirical evidence collected over the years has demonstrated that societies and cultures vary in their mean level of SWB (Diener et al., 2018; Veenhoven, 2018). For instance, research findings gathered in the World Database of Happiness (Veenhoven 2019) on SWB across nations show that, in general, European countries and Canada show higher SWB level than do the USA and East Asian countries, and that these countries, in turn, show higher SWB than African countries. Veenhoven (1989), Diener et al. (1995), and Stevenson and Wolfers (2008) found correlations of .65, .58, and .82, respectively, between cognitive SWB and gross domestic product. Diener et al. (2010) found that the happiest societies are wealthy and economically developed, and this finding was replicated across nations and within nations (Lawless & Lucas, 2011; Veenhoven, 2018). So, in general, people from the most affluent societies tend to show a higher level of life satisfaction (cognitive SWB). However, the correlation of affective SWB with GDP is smaller (r = .35) (Diener et al., 2018).

Empirical research also found that SWB was related to strong support for the rule of law and human rights, less corruption, efficient and effective governments, more progressive taxation systems, more political freedoms, less unemployment, better healthcare systems, and healthier natural environments (see Diener et al., , 2015; Oishi et al., 2018; Veenhoven, 2018, for reviews).

Cross-cultural research also found that the relationship between SWB and relevant work-related variables, such as gender equality, income equality, intrinsic motivation, trust in organizations, and trust in people vary across nations (Veenhoven, 2019). For instance, although SWB and job satisfaction correlated in most of the countries, there are also variations in the degree of the association between these two variables, and the differences can be partially attributed to cultural norms and values (Diener et al., 2018).

In summary, research conducted over decades and in more than a hundred countries shows that they vary in their levels of overall SWB, cognitive SWB, and affective SWB (Diener et al., 2020; Veenhoven, 2018, 2019).

Besides the differences in the level of SWB across countries, there are also cross-cultural differences that may influence job performance evaluations (Festing & Barzantny, 2008; Festing & Knappert, 2014; Fletcher & Perry, 2001). For example, task performance has been the core of performance assessments over the years in Western countries (Aguinis, 2019; Borman et al., 2017), but this appears not to be true in Japan, according to Nimura et al. (2000), where organizational citizenship behaviors are more explicitly expected and assessed. Related to this, Redding (1990) pointed out that the primary objective in many overseas Chinese companies is to maintain family control of the business and, consequently, performance evaluations tend to favor employees that support the family over employees that challenge the family’s authority. The organizational objectives can also differ substantially in Western and Chinese organizations, depending mainly on whether the ownership is public or private (Festing, & Knappert, 2014). Hempel (2001) mentions that Chinese state companies often had to fulfill political objectives, such as maximizing employment, and that human resources practices tend to serve these objectives.

Some studies have shown that managers might use the same rating instrument similarly across cultures, but they interpret performance differently. In a study conducted in Canada, Spain, and South Korea, Ployhart et al. (2003) found that although supervisors used the performance rating scales similarly across the three countries, they also showed cultural differences in how they perceived performance. In other words, performance may be rated equivalently, but there are significant cultural differences in how performance is perceived. Bacouel-Jentjens and Brandl (2015) conducted a study comparing the responses of subsidiaries in France and Germany to the performance appraisal system from their U.S. parent company. They found that French employees focused firmly on the performance criteria and the evaluation process, while the German employees were more focused on the appraisal outcome. As well as this, in both countries, employees showed concerns about the meaning and usefulness of the American job performance evaluation system in their countries. Bacouel-Jentjens and Brandl (2015) concluded that, in Germany, the judgments in the performance appraisal were clear and explicit, while they were covert and implicit in France, leaving room for interpretation

In a study carried out in Hong Kong, Hempel (2001) compared the performance views of Chinese and British supervisors working in the same organization. Hempel (2001) found that Chinese and U.K. performance views focused on noteworthily different aspects. For instance, Chinese supervisors used more personal attributions, notably obedience, while U.K. supervisors used significantly more behavioral or skill-based attributions. Chinese and U.K. supervisors also differed significantly in the requirements of minimum supervision and communication skills. While the Chinese supervisors did not see these factors as relevant, they are critical for the U.K. supervisors.

Freedle and Kostin (1997) found that cultural differences in the use and interpretation of common words (e.g., gullible, credulous) can lead to differential item functioning. This feature can critically affect both SWB measures (particularly, PA and NA adjective-based scales) and performance assessments. For example, Chinese managers tend to define performance in terms of personal traits such as loyalty, obedience, and morality, which contrast with the Western view more focused on outcomes (Aguinis, 2019; Hempel, 2001).

There are not only differences in how performance is defined, but also in how it is evaluated. For example, Hempel (2001) found that job performance is evaluated through essays in Hong Kong, but this method is rarely used in Western companies, where rating scales are mainly used (Aguinis, 2019; Borman et al., 2017; Viswesvaran et al., 2001). Besides, the interrater reliability of the essays is very low, mainly due to their unstructured format (Aguinis, 2019). The method of performance measurement is relevant, as it was found that the interrater reliability of supervisory ratings varies according to their purpose (e.g., research vs. administrative purposes) and the nature (e.g., mono-item scales, multi-item scales) (Salgado & Moscoso, 1996, 2019a; Salgado et al., 2016). Finally, there are also critical differences across countries in employment policies, regulations, and the legal environments that affect what, when, how, and by whom job performance is evaluated (see Shen et al., 2017, for a review of such practices in 22 African, American, European, Asian, and Pacific-region countries).

Based on the above considerations of the cross-cultural and national differences that might affect SWB and performance definitions, evaluations, and measures, it would not be a surprise if there were cross-cultural differences in the relationship between SWB and job performance. However, this issue has not been systematically examined until now. Thus, research is required to shed some light on this issue. Therefore, we posit the following research question:

Research Question 1: Is the magnitude of the relationship between SWB and job performance the same across nations and cultures?

Moderator Effect of the Cross-sectional vs. Longitudinal Design Type on the SWB-Job Performance Relationship

Research on the SWB-job performance relationship used two types of design. Some studies were cross-sectional, providing concurrent validity evidence, while others were longitudinal, providing predictive validity evidence (Boehm & Lyubomirsky, 2008; Moscoso & Salgado, 2021; Walsh et al., 2018). In the first case, SWB and job performance were evaluated at the same time or with a minimal lag (e.g., with an interval of a couple of weeks). In the second case, job performance was evaluated some months later, and in some cases, three, four, or five years later than the evaluation of SWB. For example, Cropanzano and Wright (1999), Salgado et al. (2019), and Wright and Staw (1999) used a longitudinal design, while Shaw and Gupta (2001), Wright et al. (2007), and Haider et al. (2018) used a cross-sectional design. In our database, the majority of the studies used a cross-sectional design (81.8% for cognitive SWB, 73.8% for affective SWB, and 78.6% for overall SWB).

It is well-known that cross-sectional designs are weaker when it comes to establishing causal links (Boehm & Lyubomirski, 2008; Walsh et al., 2018). On the other hand, longitudinal designs cannot control for the effects of transient error and, sometimes, produce smaller effect sizes. Also, they cannot exclude the possibility that the relationship between SWB and job performance could be due to a third variable, correlated with SWB (Walsh et al., 2018).

In other research areas, there is some evidence that the type of design is a powerful moderator of the correlation. For example, in the personality domain, Ones et al. (2012) found that the validity of integrity tests was 19% larger for concurrent designs. In the area of personnel interviews, Salgado and Moscoso (1995) found that the validity was 33% larger for the concurrent designs. Pearlman et al. (1980), in their meta-analysis of clerical ability tests, mentioned that the mean validity in predictive designs was slightly larger than the average validity of concurrent designs for five test types; they found the reverse for four test types, and the means were mostly the same in the two designs for one test type. In the case of job satisfaction, Judge et al. (2001) found that the relationship between job satisfaction and job performance was moderated by the study design (cross-sectional vs. longitudinal), being 35% larger for the concurrent designs.

In the SWB domain, Cropanzano and Wright (1999) and Salgado et al. (2019) found that the correlation between SWB and job performance was larger for the concurrent designs. However, this moderating effect has not been systematically examined in the SWB-job performance relationship.

Beyond the fact that the correlation size between SWB and job performance may be different depending on the nature of the design (i.e., cross-sectional vs. longitudinal), the examination of this potential difference is important to provide further clarification about two rival hypotheses on the causal direction between SWB and job performance (Boehm & Lyubomirsky, 2008; Walsh et al., 2018). The first hypothesis is that hard work and higher performance determine SWB, i.e., they are antecedents of SWB. Boehm and Lyubomirsky (2008) proposed the alternative hypothesis that SWB affects career success (including job performance).The longitudinal and experimental literature reviewed by Boehm and Lyubomirsky (2008), and Walsh et al. (2018) suggested that the alternative hypothesis might be as plausible as the one that suggests that performance precedes SWB.

None of the previous meta-analyses (i.e., Erdogan, et al., 2012; Ford et al., 2011; Moscoso & Salgado, 2021; Shockley et al., 2012) examined the potential moderating effects of the nature of the design, or the potential causal precedence of SWB over job performance, as suggested by Lyubomirsky and colleagues (Boehm & Lyubomirsky, 2008; Walsh et al., 2018). Based on the mentioned findings, we state our second research question:

Research Question 2: Does the study type (cross-sectional vs. longitudinal) moderate the validity of SWB and its components as predictors of job performance?

Main Objectives

This study has three main goals: (1) to estimate the relationship between global SWB and its components with job performance; (2) to estimate the validity of SWB across countries/regions as a predictor of job performance; and (3) to examine the moderator effect of the study type (cross-sectional vs. longitudinal). In order to achieve these goals, we conducted a series of psychometric meta-analyses.

Method

Literature Search

Electronic searches, examination of the references of previous meta-analyses, and contact with international researchers were used to locate the studies included in this meta-analysis. The electronic searches in the main databases (e.g., Psyclit, Google Scholar, ERIC) provided 1387 references. We examined the full content of each document and we excluded the majority of them because (a) they did not provide a correlation coefficient between SWB and job performance, (b) they provided a multiple correlation but not bivariate correlations, (c) they provided only the significant correlations, (d) the measure of job performance was obtained from subordinates, and (e) some valid documents were excluded because they were duplicated. This strategy provided 50 usable documents (58 independent samples). The examination of the references of previous meta-analyses and the references of the collected documents to identify potential papers were not included in the previous set. This strategy provided seven documents (eight independent samples). Finally, we contacted 15 international researchers from the USA, the United Kingdom, Germany, Italy, The Netherlands, Belgium, Spain, and France to obtain previously unidentified papers. We obtained six documents (six independent samples) with this strategy. Therefore, the final database consisted of 69 documents (articles, unpublished papers, and unpublished doctoral dissertations) with 78 independent samples.

For the Meta-Analysis Reporting Standards (MARS) specified in the Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association (2020; available at https://apastyle.apa.org/manual/related/JARS-MARS.pdf) and the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analysis (PRISMA), Table 1 contains the following information from each study for each job performance category: (1) study source, (2) SWB component, (3) country, and (4) design study (concurrent vs. longitudinal).

Table 1.  List of Studies for the SWB-Job Performance Meta-analysis 

Note. CSWB = cognitive subjective well-being; ASWB = affective subjective well-being; OSWB = overall subjective well-being.

The period covered by this meta-analysis was 27 years (1993-2020). The list of the documents appears in the section of references. Table 1 contains the list of studies and the dataset.

The Bioethics Committee of our university has declared the meta-analyses carried out with published and unpublished studies exempt from approval if they do not include personal identification data.

Inter-Coder Agreement

The two authors of this study, both with experience conducting and publishing meta-analyses, coded all correlation coefficients independently. For each study, we compared eleven categories of data points: (a) sample size, (b) correlation coefficient, (c) SWB measure, (i.e., cognitive SWB, affective SWB, and overall SWB), (d) design type (longitudinal vs. cross-sectional), (e) predictor reliability, (f) job performance reliability, (g) published vs. unpublished study, (h) study year, and (i) country. To establish the level of inter-coder agreement, we identified the number of data points and the number of disagreements. There were 870 data points in this meta-analysis, with 838 agreements and 32 disagreements, yielding a 96.4% level of agreement. Disagreements were resolved by referring back to the studies and discussion between the two authors until consensus was reached.

Identification of Outliers

It is widely accepted among meta-analysts that meta-analytic estimates (i.e., average correlation and variance) can be biased by extreme or erroneous data points (Huffcutt & Arthur, 1995; Schmidt & Hunter, 2015; Webster, 2019). An outlier is a data point that seems to be well separated from the remaining data points of the distribution, that is, a data point that yields an atypical result (Viechtbauer & Cheung, 2010; Webster, 2019; Whetzel et al., 2014). Outliers have two main effects in meta-analysis. First, they slightly change the average effect size estimate, and second, they increase the observed variability (i.e., observed variance and SD) substantially.

In this meta-analysis, we used two outlier criteria. The first outlier criterion considered the distribution of the results reported in the individual studies, that is, a random arrangement of the values around the mean effect size. Then, we calculated the units of standard deviation that each effect size deviated from the average effect size found in each meta-analysis (Wilcox, 2014). We treated a study as an outlier when three or more units of standard deviation separated each result from the mean. Therefore, based on the first criterion, we considered that a validity estimate was a potential outlier if the probability of being a member of the distribution was p < .001. Our second criterion was the sample adjusted meta-analytic deviance statistic (SAMD) by Huffcutt and Arthur (1995; see also Beal et al., 2002). To the best of our knowledge, SAMD is the only outlier detection statistic developed to be used explicitly in the meta-analysis of correlations. This technique is based on the same logic as other well-known influence statistics, such as Cook’s distance and the DFFITS (Huffcutt & Arthur, 1995; Viechtbauer & Cheung, 2010). However, the SAMD considers the sample size (i.e., sampling error), which is not considered in typical bivariate influence statistics as they assume that all data points have equal status. Moreover, Beal et al. (2002) showed that the SAMD statistic presents a normal distribution when the average sample size per study is greater than 30 individuals, the average r is less than .50, and the number of studies is greater than 5, with a bias of less than .02. As SAMD distributes normally, Beal et al. (2002) suggested that a cutoff point of 1.96 can be used to decide what correlation constitutes an outlier. We have programmed the SAMD formula in Excel to carry out this analysis. To summarize, the two outlier criteria used in the current study are (1) (|routlier - r|) / SD ≥ 3 and (2) SAMD.

The two outlier criteria agreed as they identified two studies, with four validity coefficients, as potential outliers. They were the study of Moradi et al. (2014), which reported three validity coefficients of -.17, -.14, and -.17, and the study of Singh et al., (2012), which reported a validity coefficient of -.32. The corresponding SAMD statistics were 4.07, 4.65, 4.87, and 11.16, all considerably larger than 1.96. The SD units below the mean were 3.17, 3.41, 5.75, and 4.42, respectively, all of them larger than 3 SD units. Therefore, the studies of Moradi et al. (2014) and Sing et al. (2012) were considered outliers in this study, and meta-analyses were done with and without these two studies. The effects of these two outliers were substantial on the observed variance adding artifactual variance, as can be seen in Table 2.

Publication Bias

Rothstein et al. (2005) defined publication bias as “the term for what occurs whenever the research that appears in the published literature is systematically unrepresentative of the population of completed studies” (p. 1). Schmidt and Hunter (2015) posited that a critical issue of all meta-analyses is the potential bias due to the availability of one kind of study only (e.g., published studies, significant studies). Although we have made a significant effort to include as many published and unpublished studies as possible, the average estimates could be affected by some kind of publication bias. Following the recommendations of Borenstein (2005), Kepes et al. (2012), and Schmidt and Hunter (2015), this meta-analysis used three methods of detecting potential publication bias: (a) subgroup comparison of published and unpublished studies, (b) Pearson correlation between the sample size and the effect size, and (c) subgrouping a cumulative meta-analysis with a forest plot of the average estimates of the average validity.

Table 2.  Effects of the Outliers on the Observed Variance of the Meta-analysis 

Artifact Distributions

SWB reliability. The reliability of overall SWB, cognitive SWB, and affective SWB was estimated using the internal consistency reported in the primary studies. When a study did not report the reliability, we used the average value of the reliability distribution of all combined studies (rxx = .81) as the study reliability. Next, we developed an empirical distribution for overall SWB, Cognitive SWB, and affective SWB. Descriptive statistics of the specific distributions for each meta-analysis appear in Table 3.

Table 3.  Predictor Reliability Distributions 

Note. SWB = subjective well-being.

Job performance reliability. In the studies included in the dataset, job performance was assessed with self-reports and/or supervisor ratings. As some studies did not provide the reliability of job performance, we developed an empirical distribution of internal consistency coefficients for self-reports of job performance. In the case of the supervisor ratings, we developed an empirical distribution considering the nature of the supervisor ratings. Salgado and Moscoso (2019a) found that the purpose of the rating (research vs. administrative) is a powerful moderator of interrater reliability. We used the distributions developed by Salgado and Moscoso (2019a) according to the following rules: (1) if the supervisor rating were collected for research purposes, we used .61 as an estimate of the interrater reliability; (2) if the supervisor ratings were collected for administrative purposes, we used an estimate of .48 as interrater reliability. If the job performance measure was a self-report, and the study did not provide the coefficient, we used the mean value of the distribution as the reliability (.80). Table 4 shows the descriptive statistics of the reliability distributions for each meta-analysis conducted in this study. As can be seen, the average coefficients ranged from .61 to .81.

Table 4.  Reliability Distributions of Job Performance 

Note. SWB = subjective well-being.

Results

Publication Bias Analysis

Table 5 reports the results of the publication bias analyses. As can be seen, the total of published and unpublished studies, on average, show the same effect size (.238 vs. 234). Therefore, we can reject the idea that the source of publication distorts the average validity.

Table 5.  Comparison of the Average Correlation for Published and Unpublished Studies 

Note. rw = average weighted-sample correlation; N = sample size; K = no. of studies; SWB = subjective well-being.

The second approach to detecting publication bias was to obtain the Pearson correlation between the sample size and the correlation size for SWB (all studies combined), cognitive SWB, affective SWB, and overall SWB. The correlations were -.12, -.03, -.11, and .03, respectively, and none of them was statistically significant. Therefore, the sample size does not affect the correlation size.

Our third method of analyzing publication bias was cumulative meta-analysis (CMA). There is broad agreement that CMA is the most robust technique for detecting publication bias (Borenstein, 2005; Kepes et al., 2012; Schmidt & Hunter, 2015). CMA consists of a series of meta-analyses in which new correlations are added one-by-one and a new average correlation is obtained with the addition of each new validity. Borenstein (2005) and Schmidt and Hunter (2015) posited that the studies should be ranked from the study with the largest sample size to the study with the smallest sample size. In the present case, the average correlation and its standard error are calculated, and a moving forest plot is developed. The absence of publication bias is observed when a sustained line is described after studies are added.

On the contrary, if the addition of small-sample studies changed the average correlation and its standard error, this would be evidence of publication bias. We developed cumulative forest plots for the meta-analyses of SWB. Figures 1 to 4 present the results of the CMAs for all studies combined and the three cross-cultural regions (i.e., USA, Europe, and Asia-Pacific region). As can be seen, they showed evidence against publication bias for SWB-job performance relationships. The point estimate established very rapidly in all the cases, and it did not shift with the inclusion of additional studies. Also, we carried out CMAs for all the combination SWB-job-performance-moderators, and the results were similar.

Figure 1.  Forest-Plot CMA for All Studies Combined. 

Figure 2.  Forest-Plot CMA for Studies Carried Out in the USA. 

Figure 3.  Forest-Plot CMA for the Studies of the Asia-Pacific Region. 

Figure 4.  Forest-Plot CMA for Studies Carried Out in Europe. 

As a whole, the results of the approaches used concur in that publication bias is not a relevant issue in this meta-analysis.

Meta-analysis of Cognitive SWB, Affective SWB, Overall SWB, and Job Performance Relationship

The first goal of this meta-analysis was to estimate the relationship between overall SWB, cognitive SWB and affective SWB with job performance. Table 6 reports the main results for the SWB-job performance relationships when the specific SWB estimate is not considered (SWB all studies combined), and for cognitive SWB, affective SWB, and overall SWB. The first two columns show the number of studies (K) and the total sample size (N). The next three columns present the mean observed correlation (rw), the observed variance (S2r), and the observed SDr. The next three columns show the operational validity (ρop), i.e., the observed correlation corrected by measurement error in job performance, the true correlation (ρt), and the standard deviation of the true correlation. Finally, the last three columns show the percentage of variance accounted for by the artifactual errors, the 95% confidence interval, and the 90% credibility value. For each SWB estimate, the correlations between SWB and job performance were pooled across moderators and cross-cultural regions. Therefore, this is the most global level of analysis on the SWB-job performance relationship. In what follows, we will concentrate our comments on the true correlation estimates as, in general, those comments can be applied to the observed correlation and the operational validity.

Table 6.  Results of the Meta-analysis for the Relationships of SWB-Job Performance 

Note. SWB = subjective well-being; K = number of independent samples; N = total sample size; rw= mean observed validity; S2r = sample size weighted observed variance of the correlations; SDr = standard deviation of observed validity; ρop = operational validity; ρt = true correlation (validity corrected for criterion and predictor reliability); SDρ = standard deviation of ρ; % VE = percentage of variance accounted for by artifactual errors; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval of the true correlation; 90% CV = 90% credibility value based on the true correlation.

As can be seen, the number of studies and the sample size vary considerably across the four categories. When all the studies were combined in a single category, the number of studies was practically twice the number of studies for cognitive SWB and affective SWB and five times the number of studies that used an overall measure of SWB. The true correlation estimates show that SWB and its components have a moderately large relationship with job performance, although the relationship is more robust for affective SWB (ρt = .37) than for cognitive SWB (ρt = .27). The relationship for overall SWB is ρt = .37, which is similar to the size for the affective SWB, and also more substantial than for cognitive SWB. When the type of SWB measures is ignored and all the studies are grouped in a single category, the results show that the correlation with job performance is .33.

Table 7.  Results of the Meta-analysis for the Relationships of SWB and Job Performance across Countries/Regions 

Note. SWB = subjective well-being; K = number of independent samples; N = total sample size; rw= mean observed validity; S2r = sample size weighted observed variance of the correlations; SDr = standard deviation of observed validity; ρop = operational validity; ρt = true correlation (validity corrected for criterion and predictor reliability); SDρ = standard deviation of ρ; % VE = percentage of variance accounted for by artifactual errors; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval of the true correlation; 90% CV = 90% credibility value based on the true correlation.

As a whole , true correlations show that overall SWB, cognitive SWB and affective SWB are predictors of job performance. The 95% CI bounds are close to the true correlation in the four cases, which means that the relationship is strongly supported in the present sample of studies.

The 90% credibility value (lowest bound of the 80% credibility interval) is positive in all the cases, and it indicates that the correlation generalized across studies and regions, with at least that value in 90% of the cases. Nevertheless, the width of the CrI is large enough to suggest that moderators can affect the estimated relationship between SWB and job performance. The percentage of explained variance and the width of the CrI suggest that a search for moderators is appropriate in the case of the SWB-job performance relationships.

Meta-analysis by Cross-cultural Regions

The first Research Question asked whether the SWB-job performance relationship varied across nations and cultures. To answer this question, we clustered the collected studies in three big groups of nations/cultures: USA-Canada, Europe, and the Asia-Pacific region. The first group included the studies conducted with samples from the USA and Canada plus a study conducted in Barbados. The European group included the studies conducted in the European countries (i.e., Albania, Austria, Germany, Italy, The Netherlands, Portugal, Romania, Spain, and Sweden). The third group is more heterogeneous than the two previous, as it included studies conducted in Asian countries, the Middle East, and Pacific regions (i.e., Australia, China, India, Iran, Japan, Malaysia, New Zealand, Pakistan, Singapore, and South Korea). As a whole, 23 countries were represented in this meta-analysis.

Table 7 reports the results of the meta-analyses for the three big cross-cultural regions. The order of the columns in Table 7 is the same as in the previous Table. The group USA-Canada is the most represented in the database and contains 35 studies. Europe and the Asia-Pacific region are similarly represented with 19 and 21 studies, respectively. However, the total sample size is bigger in Europe than in the other two groups.

The true correlation estimates show that the relationship between SWB and job performance is stronger for the Asia-Pacific region (ρt = .41) than for Europe (ρt = .33) and USA-Canada (ρt = .26). These three estimates indicate that the answer to Research Question 3 is that there are substantial cross-cultural differences in the SWB-job performance relationship. The bounds of the 95% CI indicate that the relationship is strongly supported in the present sample of studies. Therefore, cross-cultural differences are a relevant factor in a comprehensive explanatory model of the SWB-job performance relationship.

In relation to the observed variability of the correlations, the Asia-Pacific region shows more observed variance (S2 = .0224) than Europe (S2 = .0073), and the USA-Canada region (S2 = .0166), which is not surprising if one considers that the Asia-Pacific region included the most heterogeneous group of countries, with more diversity in religions, political systems, and socio-economic variables. Consequently, the percentage of explained variance is relatively small for this group. Nevertheless, the 90% CV shows that the true correlation generalizes across these countries, with a value larger than .15 in 90% of cases.

In addition to the previous meta-analyses, we divided the studies carried out in Europe into two subgroups, Northern Europe and Southern Europe, and the studies carried out in the Asia-Pacific into three subgroups, India-Pakistan-Malaysia, China-Korea-Japan, and Australia-New Zealand. The results for these five subgroups also appear in Table 7.

As can be seen, the more substantial true correlations correspond to the India-Pakistan-Malaysia and China-Korea-Japan subgroups (.48 and .41, respectively). The two European subgroups showed true correlations .34 and .31 for North-European countries and South-European countries, respectively, and the true correlation for the Australia-New Zealand subgroup (ρ = .32) was similar to the one for the European subgroups. These three subgroups showed noticeably lower correlations than the India-Pakistan-Malaysia and China-Korea-Japan subgroups. The 90% CV indicated that the relationship between SWB and job performance generalizes within and across cross-cultural regions. In general, the variability was small, except for the India-Pakistan-Malaysia subgroup.

Table 8.  Results of the Meta-analysis for the Combinations of SWB-Job Performance-Design Type (Concurrent vs. Longitudinal) 

Note. SWB = subjective well-being; K = number of independent samples; N = total sample size; rw= mean observed validity; S2r = sample size weighted observed variance of the correlations; SDr = standard deviation of observed validity; ρop = operational validity; ρt = true correlation (validity corrected for criterion and predictor reliability); SDρ = standard deviation of ; % VE = percentage of variance accounted for by artifactual errors; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval of the true correlation; 90% CV = 90% credibility value based on the true correlation.

Ideally, a more fine-grained meta-analysis would be desirable, for example, at the country level, or the main religion level (for several countries). However, the number of studies per country is too small to conduct this analysis, except in the case of the USA. In this case, when the studies from Canada and Barbados were excluded from the analysis, the true correlation for the USA was the smallest of the estimates reported in Table 7t = .23, K = 32, N = 5,708).

Both the percentage of explained variance and the width of the 80% CrI suggest that an analysis of moderators would be of interest to the three cross-cultural regions, but the number of studies does not permit that analysis. Besides, the moderators might not be the same for the three regions. For example, the main religion of the country may be more relevant for the Asia-Pacific region than for Europe, as there is more variety of religions in the Asia-Pacific region than in Europe.

Meta-analysis by SWB-Design Type

Table 8 presents the results of the meta-analyses conducted to examine the potential moderator effects of the design type on the relationship between SWB and job performance. In this case, we conducted the eight meta-analyses that resulted from the combination of the four SWB estimates (i.e., all studies combined, cognitive SWB, affective SWB, and overall SWB) by the two types of designs (i.e., cross-situational and longitudinal).

The true correlation was greater for the four meta-analyses of the studies which used a cross-situational design. The differences were very small for the meta-analyses of all studies combined and for cognitive SWB, but they were more substantial for the meta-analyses of affective SWB and overall SWB. On average, the cross-sectional designs show a 13% larger true correlation than the longitudinal designs (.34 vs. .30, respectively).

On average, artifactual errors explain 84% of the observed variance of the correlations in the studies which used a longitudinal design. However, the artifactual errors accounted for 30% of the observed variance in studies that used a cross-sectional design. This result is mainly due to the fact that, on average, cross-sectional studies used samples almost twice times larger than the longitudinal studies did.

The 90% CVs were positive and considerably different from zero in all the cases, which indicates that the relationship of SWB and its components generalizes across studies for both the cross-sectional and longitudinal designs. However, the size of the generalizability is larger for the longitudinal designs than for the cross-sectional designs.

Therefore, the answer to research question two is that the study design matters as it moderates the correlation of SWB and its components with job performance, although the magnitude of the design influence suggests that the design is not a critical factor. Nevertheless, the results do not permit us to establish that there is a stronger relationship in one direction that in the other (i.e. job performance to SWB vs. SWB to job performance), as the majority of the studies contain a single measure of SWB and job performance. This limitation does not allow us to discard the possibility that the relationship between SWB could be due to a third variable correlated with SWB, or to the previous effects of SWB on job performance. A more appropriate design would require three or more SWB measures and three or more measures of job performance, appropriately separated in time. Unfortunately, our dataset does not contain these kinds of studies.

Summary of the Findings

The main finding is that SWB is moderately correlated with job performance, independently if it is measured as affective SWB, cognitive SWB, or as overall SWB. In the second place, all the meta-analytic estimates of validity showed that affective SWB was more highly correlated with job performance than cognitive SWB. In third place, the relationship between SWB and job performance varies depending on the cross-cultural regions, being higher for the Asia-Pacific region than for Europe and the USA. The findings also revealed that the type of design moderates the relationship between SWB and job performance.

Discussion

Previous meta-analyses by Erdogan et al. (2012), Ford et al. (2011), Moscoso and Salgado (2021), and Shockley et al. (2012) estimated, separately, the average correlation of the cognitive and affective components of SWB with job performance. However, they did not examine (a) the possibility that different nations and cultures might show a different degree of correlation between SWB and job performance, (b) if the two components of SWB showed similar correlation with job performance, (c) if despite the variability reported, there was evidence of generalizability in the relationships, (d) if the correlation had been influenced by potential moderators such as the design type (i.e., cross-sectional vs. longitudinal), and (e) if they were affected by publication bias. In examining these issues, this meta-analysis made several contributions.

Firstly, the meta-analysis showed that overall SWB, cognitive SWB, and affective SWB moderately correlated with job performance. This finding means that overall SWB and its two components are valid predictors of performance at work. The correlation is similar or even higher than the one found for other well-known variables related to job performance, such as the Big Five personality dimensions, cognitive abilities, emotional intelligence, structured interviews, and in-basket tests (see, for instance, the meta-analyses of Connolly & Ones, 2010; Joseph & Newman, 2010; Judge et al., 2013; Ones et al., 2012; Otero et al., 2020; Salgado, 2017; Salgado et al., 2015; Salgado et al., 2007; Salgado & Moscoso, 2019b; Salgado & Tauriz, 2014; Van Rooy & Viswesvaran, 2004; Whetzel et al., 2014). Besides, the 90% credibility values for overall SWB, cognitive SWB, and affective SWB showed evidence of international validity generalization.

Secondly, the meta-analysis contributed to clarify that there are some cross-cultural differences across countries/regions in the magnitude of the relationship between SWB and job performance. Our findings showed that the relationship was more robust in the Asia-Pacific region than in Europe and the USA-Canada region. More specifically, the findings showed that the relationship between SWB and job performance is more robust in India, Pakistan, China, Korea, and Japan than in the Northern and Southern European countries, Australia and New Zealand, the USA and Canada. In comparison with the rest of the countries/regions, the USA showed a smaller relationship. How can we account for the fact that the relationship between SWB and job performance is stronger for some countries than for others? This is a question that this meta-analysis does not permit us to answer. However, we can speculate, as Walsh et al. (2018) suggested, that the cultures that reinforce frequent low-arousal positive emotions (e.g., calm), not high-arousal ones (e.g., enthusiasm), and that simultaneously deemphasize low negative emotions (anger) can produce a stronger SWB-job performance relationship. On the other hand, we can also speculate that cultures that overemphasize hard working, reward those efforts, and emphasize long-term goals will also show a stronger SWB-job performance relationship. Another potential explanation is that the conceptions of what is high job performance vary across cultures. For instance, individualistic cultures can consider task performance more relevant, while collectivistic cultures may consider citizenship performance and commitment more relevant. The current database of studies does not permit us to go further in the required analysis, because the number of studies per cell is very small in some cases and because there are no studies in other cases. Future research should be conducted to identify the specific factors (e.g., cultural values) that determine those cross-cultural/national differences.

Thirdly, the meta-analysis contributed to clarify the moderator role of the study design on the magnitude of the validity of SWB. On average, the validity was slightly higher in the cross-sectional studies than in the longitudinal ones (.34 vs. .30). As a percentage, the difference is 13%. This finding is also relevant to the debate on the advantages and limitations of the cross-sectional vs. longitudinal designs. At least, as far as SWB validity for predicting job performance is concerned, the design type does not seem to greatly affect the magnitude of the relationship critically.

The current meta-analytic findings have implications for practice of work and organizational psychology. A potential practical implication is that SWB and its components are valid predictors of job performance across de world. This means that, despite potential cross-cultural differences, the SWB-job performance relationship is robust. Consequently, measures of SWB such as the SWLS and SPANE might be used for personnel purposes at least in the countries included in this meta-analysis.

The meta-analytic results of the longitudinal studies could have practical implications. As the meta-analysis of those studies indicates that the level of SWB in a specific time correlates with the level of job performance in a subsequent time, with the two times typically separated by months (or even years), a practical implication is that overall job performance can be improved by enlarging employee SWB. One strategy could be to include measures of SWB together with the procedures typically used in hiring processes. Considering that the relationship between job performance and SWB tends to remain relatively stable over time, using SWB measures may help to increase organizational effectiveness. Alternatively, procedures that predict the level of employee SWB might be included in the selection processes. Research has found that personality factors are valid predictors of satisfaction with life and emotional balance (Lucas, 2018; Steel et al., 2008). Therefore, these procedures can also permit the forecasting of the level of SWB of future employees.

This meta-analysis also has some limitations. A first limitation is that we were not able to find studies conducted in Central and South America and Africa. Walsh et al. (2018) also mentioned the absence of studies on SWB-job performance in those countries. Therefore, our findings cannot be generalized to those countries. Future studies should examine the research questions in African and Central and South American countries to develop a clear picture of the relationships between SWB, its components, and job performance in those countries. Based on the results of this study, we can speculate that the relationship would be similar to the ones found here, but this is just a conjecture that needs to be tested.

A second limitation is that the number of studies did not permit to conduct meta-regression analysis using, for instance, Hofstede’s cross-cultural dimensions of values (i.e., power distance, individualism, uncertainty avoidance, masculinity, long-term orientation, and indulgence). Schmidt and Hunter (2015) mention that the number of studies required for a meta-regression should be as large as the number of cases required in an individual study. This fact means about 150-200 primary studies, but we were able to collect less than 80.

In summary, this study contributes to the clarification of cross-cultural effects on the relationships between SWB and job performance across the world. It showed that the existence of a substantial relationship between SWB and job performance and that the validity generalizes across the countries. Also, the study clarifies that the design type moderates the relationship slightly.

Acknowledgements

We would like to express our profound gratitude to Ed Diener, Jim Harter, and Frank L. Schmidt for their insightful and helpful comments on previous versions of this paper.

Cite this article as: Salgado, J. F. & Moscoso, S. (2022). Cross-cultural evidence of the relationship between subjective well-being and job performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 38(1), 27-42. https://doi.org/10.5093/jwop2022a3

Funding: This research was partially supported by grant PSI2020-114984GB-100 from the Ministry of Education and Innovation to Silvia Moscoso and by grant PSI2017-87603-P from the Spanish Ministry of Education and Innovation to Jesús F. Salgado and Silvia Moscoso.

References

Adler, A., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2016). Using well-being for public policy: Theory, measurement, and recommendations. International Journal of Wellbeing, 6(1), 1-35. https://doi.org/10.5502/ijw.v6i1.429 [ Links ]

Aguinis, H. (2019). Performance management. Wiley. [ Links ]

*Ahmed, N., & Malik, B. (2019). Impact of psychological empowerment on job performance of teachers: Mediating role of psychological well-being. Review of Economics and Development Studies, 5(3), 451-460. https://doi.org/10.26710/reads.v5i3.693 [ Links ]

*Alessandri, G., Vecchione, M., Tisak, J., Deiana, G., Caria, S., & Caprara, G. V. (2012). The utility of positive orientation in predicting job performance and organisational citizenship behaviors: International review of applied psychology. Applied Psychology, 61(4), 669-698. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2012.00511.x [ Links ]

*Alonso, P., & Llovo, A. (2020). Relationships between job satisfaction, subjective well-being, and job performance. Unpublished manuscript. Department of Work Psychology, University of Santiago de Compostela, Santiago de Compostela, Spain. [ Links ]

American Psychological Association. (2020). Publication manual (7th edition). American Psychological Association. [ Links ]

*Athota, V. S., Budhwar, P., & Malik, A. (2020). Influence of personality traits and moral values on employee well-being, resilience and performance: A cross-national study. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 69(3), 653-685. https://doi.org/10.1111/apps.12198 [ Links ]

*Babin, B. J., & Boles, J. S. (1998). Employee behavior in a service environment: A model and test of potential differences between men and women. Journal of Marketing, 62(2), 77-91. https://doi.org/10.2307/1252162 [ Links ]

Bacouel-Jentjens, S., & Brandl, J. (2015). Cross-cultural responses to performance appraisals in Germany and France: A refinement of the picture. International Journal of Cross-Cultural Management, 15(3), 1-20. https://doi.org/10.1177/1470595815603412 [ Links ]

*Baranik, L. E., Wang, M., Gong, Y., & Shi, J. (2017). “Customer mistreatment, employee health, and job performance: Cognitive rumination and social sharing as mediating mechanisms”: Corrigendum. Journal of Management, 43(4), 1. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206315580581 [ Links ]

Beal, D. J., Corey, D. M., & Dunlap, W. P. (2002). On the bias of Huffcutt and Arthur’s (1995) procedure for identifying outliers in the meta-analysis of correlations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(3), 583-590. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.87.3.583 [ Links ]

Berkman, P. L. (1971). Measurement of mental health in a general population survey. American Journal of Epidemiology, 94(2), 105-111. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a121301 [ Links ]

*Bhuian, S. N., Menguc, B., & Borsboom, R. (2005). Stressors and job outcomes in sales: A triphasic model versus a linear-quadratic-interactive model. Journal of Business Research, 58(2), 141-150. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0148-2963(03)00132-2 [ Links ]

Boehm, J. K., & Kubzansky, L. D. (2012). The heart’s content: The association between positive psychological well-being and cardiovascular health. Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 655-691. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027448 [ Links ]

Boehm, J. K., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Does happiness promote career success? Journal of Career Success, 16(1), 101-116. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069072707308140 [ Links ]

Borenstein, M. (2005). Software for publication bias. In H. R. Rothstein, A. J. Sutton, & M. Borenstein (Eds.), Publication bias in meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment, and adjustments (pp. 193-220). Wiley. [ Links ]

Borman, W. C., Grossman, M. R., Bryant, R. H., & Dorio, J. (2017). The measurement of task performance as criteria in selection research. In J. L. Farr & N. T. Tippins (Eds). Handbook of personnel selection (2nd ed.) (pp. 429-447). Routledge. [ Links ]

Bowling, N. A., Eschleman, K. J., & Wan, Q. (2010). A meta-analytic examination of the relationship between job satisfaction and subjective well-being. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 83(4), 915-934. https://doi.org/10.1348/096317909x478557 [ Links ]

*Boyar, S. L., Wagner, T. A., Petzinger, A., & McKinley, R. B. (2016). The impact of family roles on employee’s attitudes and behaviors. Journal of Management Development, 35(5), 623-635. https://doi.org/10.1108/JMD-07-2015-0096 [ Links ]

*Briggs, E., Jaramillo, F., & Weeks, W. A. (2012). The influences of ethical climate and organization identity comparisons on salespeople and their job performance. Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 32(4), 421-436. https://doi.org/10.2753/PSS0885-3134320402 [ Links ]

Busseri, M. A. (2015). Toward a resolution of the tripartite structure of subjective well-being. Journal of Personality, 83(4), 413-428. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12116 [ Links ]

Busseri, M. A. (2018). Examining the structure of subjective well-being through meta-analysis of the association among positive affect, negative affect, and life satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences, 122, 68-71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.10.003 [ Links ]

Busseri, M. A., & Sadava, S. W. (2011). A review of the tripartite structure of subjective well-being: Implications for conceptualization, operationalization, and synthesis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15(3), 290-314. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868310391271 [ Links ]

Cantril, H. (1965). The pattern of human concerns. Rutgers University Press. [ Links ]

*Chughtai, A. A. (2019). A closer look at the relationship between life satisfaction and job performance. Applied Research in Quality of Life, 16, 805-825. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11482-019-09793-2 [ Links ]

*Clarke, N., & Mahadi, N. (2017). Mutual recognition respect between leaders and followers: Its relationship to follower job performance and well-being. Journal of Business Ethics, 141(1), 163-178. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-015-2724-z [ Links ]

Connelly, B. S., & Ones, D. S. (2010). An other perspective on personality: Meta-analytic integration of observers’ accuracy and predictive validity. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 1092-1122. htpps://doi.org/10.1037/a0021212 [ Links ]

*Cropanzano, R., & Wright, T. A. (1999). A 5-year of change in the relationship between well-being and job performance. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research, 51(4), 252-265. https://doi.org/10.1037/1061-4087.51.4.252 [ Links ]

*De Cuyper, N., & De Witte, H. (2006). The impact of job insecurity and contract type on attitudes, well-being and behavioural reports: A psychological contract perspective. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 79(3), 395-409. https://doi.org/10.1348/096317905X53660 [ Links ]

*De Cuyper, N., & De Witte, H. (2007). Job insecurity in temporary versus permanent workers: Associations with attitudes, well-being, and behaviour. Work & Stress, 21(1), 65-84. https://doi.org/10.1080/02678370701229050 [ Links ]

*Devonish, D. (2013). Workplace bullying, employee performance and behaviors: The mediating role of psychological well-being. Employee Relations, 35(6), 630-647. https://doi.org/10.1108/ER-01-2013-0004 [ Links ]

Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95(3), 542-575. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.542 [ Links ]

Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being: The science of happiness, and a proposal for a national index. American Psychologist, 55(1), 34-43. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.34 [ Links ]

Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2008). Happiness: Unlocking the mysteries of psychological wealth. Blackwell. [ Links ]

Diener, E., Diener, M., & Diener, C. (1995). Factors predicting subjective well-being in nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 851-864. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.851 [ Links ]

Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Research, 49(1), 71-75. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13 [ Links ]

Diener, E., Kahneman, D., Tov, W. R., & Arora, R. (2010). Income’s association with judgments of life versus feelings. In E. Diener, J. F. Helliwell, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), International differences in well-being (pp. 3-15). Oxford University Press. [ Links ]

Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R. E. (2003). Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: Emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 403-425. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145056 [ Links ]

Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R. E. (2015). National accounts of subjective well-being. American Psychologist, 70(3), 324-342. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038899 [ Links ]

Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Tay, L. (2018). Advances in subjective well-being research. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(4), 253-260. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0307-6 [ Links ]

Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Very happy people. Psychological Science, 13(1), 81-84. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00415 [ Links ]

Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Beyond money: Toward an economy of well-being. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5(1), 1-31. https.//doi.org/10.1111/j,0963-7214.2004.00501001.x [ Links ]

Diener, E., Thapa, S., & Tay, L. (2020). Positive emotions at work. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 7, 451-477. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-012119-044908 [ Links ]

Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D., Oishi, S., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2009). New measures of well-being: Flourishing and positive and negative feelings. Social Indicators Research, 39, 247-266. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-009-9493-y [ Links ]

*Donaldson, S. I., & Blanchard, M. A. (1995). The seven health practices, well-being, and performance at work: Evidence for the value of reaching small and underserved worksites. Preventive Medicine, 24(3), 270-277. https://doi.org/10.1006/pmed.1995.1044 [ Links ]

*Edgar, F., Geare, A., & Zhang, J. A. (2017). A comprehensive concomitant analysis of service employees’ well-being and performance. Personnel Review, 46(8), 1870-1889. https://doi.org/10.1108/PR-05-2016-0108 [ Links ]

Erdogan, B., Bauer, T. N., Trixillo, D. N., & Mansfield, L. R. (2012). Whistle while you work: A review of the life satisfaction literature. Journal of Management, 38(4), 1038-1083. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206311429379 [ Links ]

Festing, M., & Barzantny, C. (2008) A comparative approach to performance management in France and Germany: The impact of the European and the country-specific environment. European Journal of International Management, 2(2), 208-227. https://doi.org/10.1504/EJIM.2008.017762 [ Links ]

Festing, M., & Knappert, L (2014) Country-specific profiles of performance management in China, Germany, and the United States – an empirical test. Thunderbird International Business Review, 56(4), 331-351. https://doi.org/10.1002/tie.21631 [ Links ]

Fisher, C. D. (2010). Happiness at work. International Journal of Management Review, 12(4), 384-412. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2370.2009.00270.x [ Links ]

Fletcher, C., & Perry, E. L. (2001). Performance appraisal and feedback: Considerations of national culture and a review of contemporary research and future trends. In N. Anderson, D. S. Ones, H. K. Sinangil, & C. Viswesvaran (Eds.), Handbook of industrial, work and organizational psychology (Vol 1, pp. 127-144). Sage. [ Links ]

Ford, M. T., Cerasoli, C. P., Higgins, J. A., & Decesare, A. L. (2011). Relationships between psychological, physical, and behavioural health and work performance: A review and meta-analysis. Work & Stress, 25(3), 185-204. https://doi.org/10.1080/02678373.2011.609035 [ Links ]

Fordyce, M. W. (1988). A review of research on the happiness measures: A sixty second index of happiness and mental health. Social Indicators Research, 20(4), 355-381. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00302333 [ Links ]

Freedle, R., & Kostin, I. (1997). Predicting black and white differential item functioning in verbal analogy performance. Intelligence, 24(3), 417-444. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90058-1 [ Links ]

*Greenhaus, J. H., Bedeain, A. G., & Mossholder, K. W. (1987). Work experiences, job performance, and feelings of personal and family well-being. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 31(2), 200-215. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-8791(87)90057-1 [ Links ]

*Greguras, G. J., & Diefendorff, J. M. (2010). Why does proactive personality predict employee life satisfaction and work behaviors? A field investigation of the mediating role of the self-concordance model. Personnel Psychology, 63(3), 539-560. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.2010.01180.x [ Links ]

Guven, C. (2012). Reversing the question: Does happiness affect consumption and saving behavior? Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(4), 701-707. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep2012.01.002 [ Links ]

*Haider, S., Jabeen, S., & Ahmad, J. (2018). Moderated mediation between work life balance and employee job performance: The role of psychological well-being and satisfaction with coworkers. Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 34(1), 29-37. https://doi.org/10.5093/jwop2018a4 [ Links ]

Harter, J. K., Schmidt, F. L., Agrawal, S., Plowman, S. K., & Blue, A. T. (2020). Increased business value for positive job attitudes during economic recessions: A meta-analysis and SEM analysis. Human Performance, 33(4), 307-330. https://doi.org/10.1080/08959285.2020.1758702 [ Links ]

Harter, J. K., Schmidt, F. L., Asplund, J. W., Killham, E. A., & Agrawal, S. (2010). Causal impact of employee work perceptions on bottom line organizations. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(4), 378-389. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610374589 [ Links ]

Harter, J. K., Schmidt, F. L., & Hayes, T. L. (2002). Business unit-level relationship between employee satisfaction, employee engagement, and business outcomes. A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(2), 268-279. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.87.2.268 [ Links ]

Hempel, P. S. (2001). Differences between Chinese and Western managerial views of performance. Personnel Review, 30(2), 203-226. https://doi.org/10.1108/00483480110694525 [ Links ]

Heuchert, J. P., & McNair, D. M. (2012). POMS-2 manual: A profile of mood states. Multi-Health Systems, Inc. [ Links ]

Hills, P., & Argyle, M. (2002). The Oxford Happiness Questionnaire: A compact scale for the measurement of psychological well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 33(7), 1073-1082. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00213-6 [ Links ]

*Hosie, P., Sevastos, P., & Cooper, C.L. (2007). The “happy productive worker thesis” and Australian managers. Journal of Human Values, 13(2), 151-176. https://doi.org/10.1177/097168580701300207 [ Links ]

*Hosie, P., Sharma, P., & Kingshort, R. P. J. (2019). “Happy-performing managers’ thesis: Testing the mediating role of job-related affective outcomes on the impact of role stressors on contextual performance. International Journal of Manpower, 40(2), 356-372. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJM-04-2018-0124 [ Links ]

Huffcutt, A. I., & Arthur, W. (1995). Development of a new outlier statistic for meta-analytic data. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80(2), 327-334. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.80.2.327 [ Links ]

*Hyland, M. M. (1999). Flexibility in work arrangements: How availability, preferences, and use affect business outcomes (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). [ Links ]

*Jalali, Z., & Heidari (2016). The relationship between happiness, subjective well-being, creativity and job performance of primary school teachers in Ramhormoz City. International Education Studies, 9(6), 45-52. https://doi.org/10.5539/ies.v9n6p45 [ Links ]

*Jones, M. D. (2006). Which is a better predictor of job performance: Job satisfaction or life satisfaction? Journal of Behavioral and Applied Management, 8(1), 20-42. https://doi.org/10.21818/001c.16696 [ Links ]

Joseph, D. L., & Newman, D. A. (2010). Emotional intelligence: An integrative meta-analysis and cascading model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(1), 54-78. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017286 [ Links ]

Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Erez, A., & Locke, E. A. (2005). Core self-evaluations and job and life satisfaction: The role of self-concordance and goal attainment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(2), 257-268. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.90.2.257 [ Links ]

Judge, T. A., Rodell, J. B., Klinger, R. L., Simon, L. S., & Crawford, E. R. (2013). Hierarchical representations of the five-factor model of personality in predicting job performance: Integrating three organizing frameworks with two theoretical perspectives. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98(6), 875-925. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033901 [ Links ]

Judge, T. A., Thoresen, C. J., Bono, J. E., & Patton, G. K. (2001). The job satisfaction-job performance relationship: A qualitative and quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 127(3), 376-407. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.3.376 [ Links ]

*Junça-Silva, A., Caetano, A., & Lopes, R. R. (2017). Daily uplifts, well-being and performance in organizational settings: The differential mediating roles of affect and work engagement. Journal of Happiness Studies: An Interdisciplinary Forum on Subjective Well-being, 18, 591-606. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-016-9740-2 [ Links ]

*Karatepe, O. M., & Bekteshi, L. (2008). Antecedents and outcomes of work-family facilitation and family-work facilitation among frontline hotel employees. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 27(1), 83-109. https.//doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhm.2007.09.004 [ Links ]

Kepes, S. Banks, G. C., McDaniel, M. A., & Whetzel, D. L. (2012). Publication bias in the organizational sciences. Organizational Research Methods, 15(4), 624-662. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428112452760 [ Links ]

*Kossek, E. J., Colquitt, J. A., & Noe, R. A. (2001). Caregiving decisions, well-being, and performance: The effects of place and provider as a function of dependent type and work-family climates. Academy of Management Journal, 44(1), 29-44. https://doi.org/10.5465/3069335 [ Links ]

*Kovacs, C., Stiglbauer, B., Batinic, B., & Gnambs, T. (2018). Exploring different forms of job (dis)satisfaction and their relationship with well-being, motivation and performance: Applied Psychology: An International Review, 67(3), 523-556. https://doi.org/10.1111/apps.12128 [ Links ]

*Lado, M., Otero, I., & Salgado, J. F. (2021). Cognitive reflection, life satisfaction, emotional balance, and job performance. Psichotema, 33(1), 118-124. [ Links ]

Larsen, R. (2009). The contributions of positive and negative affect to emotional well-being. Psychological Topics, 2, 247-266. [ Links ]

*Law, K., Wong, C., Huang, G., & Li, X. (2008). The effects of emotional intelligence on job performance and life satisfaction for the research and development scientists in China. Asia Pacific Journal of Management, 25(1), 51-69. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10490-007-9062-3 [ Links ]

Lawless, N. M., & Lucas, R. E. (2011). Predictors of regional well-being: A county-level analysis. Social Indicators Research, 101, 341-357. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-010-9667-7 [ Links ]

*Levine, E.L., Zu, X., Yang, L-Q, Ispas, D., Pitariu, H., Bian, R., Ding, D., Capotescu, R., Che, H., & Musat, S. (2011). Cross-national explorations of the impact of affect at work using the state-trait emotion measure: A coordinate series of studies in the United States, China, and Romania. Human Performance, 24(5), 405-442. https://doi.org/10.1080/08959285.2011.614302 [ Links ]

*Li, L. J. (2009). An extended model of expatriate effectiveness: The role of social networks (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). [ Links ]

*Lim, D. H., Song, J. H., & Choi, M. (2012). Work-family interface: Effect of enrichment and conflict on job performance of Korean workers. Journal of Management & Organization, 18(3), 383-397. https://doi.org/10.5172/jmo.2012.18.3.383 [ Links ]

Lucas, R. E. (2018). Exploring the associations between personality and subjective well-being. In E. Diener, S. Oishi, & L. Tay (Eds.), Handbook of well-being. UT: DEF Publishers. [ Links ]

Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131(6), 803-855. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.803 [ Links ]

Lyubomirsky, S. L., & Lepper, H. S. (1999). A measure of subjective happiness: Preliminary reliability and construct validation. Social Indicators Research, 46, 137-155. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1006824100041 [ Links ]

*Magnier-Watanabe, R., Uchida, T., Orsini, P., & Benton, C. (2017). Organizational virtuousness and job performance in Japan: Does happiness matter? International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 25(4), 628-646. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJOA-10-2016-1074 [ Links ]

*Merriman, K. K. (2016). Extrinsic work values and feedback: Contrary effects for performance and well-being. Human Relations, 70(3), 339-361. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018726716655391 [ Links ]

Moore, S., & Diener, E. (2019). Types of subjective well-being and their associations with relationship outcomes. Journal of Positive Psychology & Wellbeing, 3(2), 112-118. [ Links ]

*Moradi, S., Nima, A., Ricciardi, M. R., Archer, T., & García, D. (2014). Exercise, character strengths, well-being, and learning climate in the prediction of performance over a 6-month period at a call center. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, a497. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00497 [ Links ]

Moscoso, S., & Salgado, J. F. (2021). Meta-analytic examination of a suppressor effect on subjective well-being and job performance relationship. Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 37(2), 119-131. https://doi.org/10.5093/jwop2021a13 [ Links ]

Nimura, H., Ishashiro, S., & Naito, J. (2000). A meta-analysis and validity generalization study of a personnel test and a general cognitive test for measuring managerial aptitude. Japanese Journal of Administrative Science, 13, 159-167. https://doi.org/10.5651/jaas.13.159 [ Links ]

*Odle-Dusseau, H. N. (2008). Organizational and family resources as predictors of well-being, family functioning, and employee performance: a longitudinal study (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). [ Links ]

Oishi, S., Kushlev, K., & Schmimmack, U. (2018). Progressive taxation, income inequality, and happiness. American Psychologist, 73(2), 157-168. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000166 [ Links ]

Ones, D. S., Viswesvaran, C., & Schmidt, F. L. (2012). Integrity tests predict counterproductive work behaviors and job performance well: Comment on Van Iddekinge, Roth, Raymark, and Odle-Dusseau (2012). Journal of Applied Psychology, 97(3), 537-542. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024825 [ Links ]

Otero, I., Cuadradpo, D., & Marínez, A. (2020). Convergent and predictive validity of the Big Five assessed with single-stimulus and quasi-ipsative questionnaires. Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 36(3), 212-222. https://doi.org/10.5093/jwop2020a17 [ Links ]

Pearlman, K., Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1980). Validity generalization results for tests used to predict job proficiency and training success in clerical occupations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 65(4), 569-607. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.65.4.373 [ Links ]

Ployhart, R. E., Wiechmann, D., Schmitt, N., Sacco, J. M., & Rogg, K. (2003). The cross-cultural equivalence of job performance ratings. Human Performance, 16(1), 49-79. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327043HUP1601_3 [ Links ]

Pujol-Cols, L., & Lazzaro-Salazar, M. (2021). Ten years of reserach on psychological risks, health, and performance in Latin America: A comprehensive systematic review and research agenda. Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 37(3), 187-202. https://doi.org/10.5093/jwop2021a18 [ Links ]

*Rabenu, E., Yaniv, E., & Elizur, D. (2017). The relationship between psychological capital, coping with stress, well-being, and performance. Current Psychology: A Journal for Diverse Perspectives on Diverse Psychological Issues, 36, 875-887. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-016-9477-4 [ Links ]

Redding, S. G. (1990). The spirit of Chinese capitalism. De Gruyter. [ Links ]

*Rego, A., & Cunha, M. P. E. (2008). Authentizotic climates and employee happiness: Pathways to individual performance? Journal of Business Research, 61(7), 739-752. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2007.08.003 [ Links ]

*Riaz, M. N., Riaz, M. A., & Batool, N. (2014). Managerial decision-making styles as predictors of personal and organizational outcomes of in-service employees. Journal of Behavioural Sciences, 24(2), 100-116. [ Links ]

Rothstein, H. R., Sutton, A. J., & Borenstein, M. (2005). Publication bias in meta-analysis. In Rothstein, H. R., Sutton, A. J., & Borenstein, M. (Eds.), Publication bias in meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment, and adjustments (pp. 1-7). Wiley. [ Links ]

Sagi, L., Bareket-Bojmel, L., Tziner, A., Icekson, T., & Mordoch, T. (2021). Social support and well-being among relocating women: The mediating roles of resilence and optimism. Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 37(2), 107-117. https://doi.org/10.5093/jwop2021a11 [ Links ]

Salgado, J. F. (2017). Moderator effects of job complexity on the validity of forced-choice personality inventories for predicting job performance. Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 33(3), 229-238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rpto.2017.07.001 [ Links ]

Salgado, J. F., Anderson, N., & Táuriz, G. (2015). The validity of ipsative and quasi-ipsative forced-choice personality inventories for different occupational groups: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 88(4), 797-834. https://doi.org/10.1111/joop.12098 [ Links ]

*Salgado, J. F., Blanco, S., & Moscoso, S. (2019). Subjective well-being and job performance: Testing of a suppressor effect. Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 35(2), 93-102. https://doi.org/10.5093/jwop2019a9 [ Links ]

Salgado, J. F., Gorriti, M., & Moscoso, S. (2007). La entrevista conductual estructura y el desempeño laboral en la Administración Pública española: propiedades psicométricas y reacciones de justicia. Revista de Psicología del Trabajo y de las Organizaciones, 23(1), 39-55. [ Links ]

*Salgado, J. F., & Lado, M. (2018). Subjective well-being, satisfaction with life, positive and negative affect, and job performance. Unpublished manuscript. University of Santiago de Compostela. [ Links ]

Salgado, J. F., & Moscoso, S. (1995). Validez de la entrevista conductual estructurada. Revista de Psicología del Trabajo y de las Organizaciones, 11, 9-24. [ Links ]

Salgado, J. F., & Moscoso, S. (1996). Meta-analysis of the interrater reliability of job performance ratings in validity studies of personnel selection. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 83(3_suppl), 1195-1201. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1996.83.3f.1195 [ Links ]

Salgado, J. F., & Moscoso, S. (2019a). Meta-analysis of interrater reliability of supervisory performance ratings: Effects of the appraisal purpose, range restriction, and scale type. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, e2281. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02281 [ Links ]

Salgado, J. F., & Moscoso, S. (2019b). Meta-analysis of the validity of general mental ability for five performance criteria: Hunter and Hunter (1984) revisited. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, e2227. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02227 [ Links ]

*Salgado, J. F., & Moscoso, S. (2020). Experiencia del tele-trabajo en municipios asturianos de menos de 20.000 habitantes. Consorcio Asturiano de Servicios Tecnológicos (CAST). Oviedo, Spain. [ Links ]

Salgado, J. F., Moscoso, S., & Anderson, N.R. (2016). Corrections for criterion reliability in validity generalization: The consistency of Hermes, the utility of Midas. Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 32(1), 17-23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rpto.2015.12.001 [ Links ]

*Salgado, J. F., Moscoso, S., Lado, M., Alonso, P., Cuadrado, D., Otero, I., & Martínez, A. (2020). COVID-19 crisis, economic stress, employee commitment, and psychological well-being. Unpublished manuscript. University of Santiago de Compostela. [ Links ]

Salgado, J. F., & Táuriz, G. (2014). The five-factor model, forced-choice personality inventories and performance: A comprehensive meta-analysis of academic and occupational validity studies. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 23(1), 3-30. https://doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2012.716198 [ Links ]

*Sargent, L. D., & Terry, D. J. (1998). The effects of work control and job demands on employee adjustment and work performance. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 71(3), 219-236. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8325.1998.tb00674.x [ Links ]

Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (2015). Methods of meta-analysis (3rd edition). Sage. [ Links ]

*Shaw, J. D., & Gupta, N. (2001). Pay fairness and employee outcomes: Exacerbation and attenuation effects of financial need. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 74(3), 299-320. https://doi.org/10.1348/096317901167370 [ Links ]

Shen, W., Sackett, P. R., Lievens, F., Schollaert, E., & Van Hoye, G. (2017). Updated perspectives on the international legal environment for selection. In J. L. Farr & N. T. Tippins (eds.). Handbook of personnel selection (2nd edition) (pp. 659-677). Pergamon. [ Links ]

Shockley, K. M., Ispas, D., Rossi, M. E., & Levine, E. E. (2012). A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between state affect, discrete emotions, and job performance. Human Performance, 25(5), 377-411. https://doi.org/10.1080/08959285.2012.721832 [ Links ]

*Singh, P., Suar, D., & Leiter, M. P. (2012). Antecedents, work-related consequences, and buffers of job burnout among Indian software developers. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 19(1), 83-104. https://doi.org/10.1177/1548051811429572 [ Links ]

*Sora Miana, B., González-Morales, M. G., Caballer, A., & Peiró, J. M. (2011). Consequences of job insecurity and the moderator role of occupational group. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 14(2), 820-831. https://doi.org/10.5209/rev_SJOP.2011.v14.n2.29 [ Links ]

*Staw, B. M., & Barsade, S. G. (1993). Affect and managerial performance: A test of the sadder-but-wiser vs. happier-and-smarter hypotheses. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38(2), 304-331. https://doi.org/10.2307/2393415 [ Links ]

*Staw, B. M., Sutton, R. I., & Pelled, L. H. (1994). Employee positive emotion and favorable outcomes at the workplace. Organization Science, 5(1), 51-71. https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.5.1.51 [ Links ]

Steel, P., Schmidt, J., & Shultz, J. (2008). Refining the relationship between personality and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 138-161. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.138 [ Links ]

Stevensons, B., & Wolfers, J. (2008). Economic growth and happiness: Reassessing the Easterlin paradox. Brooking Papers on Economic Act, 39, 1-102. https://doi.org/10.3386/w14282 [ Links ]

Tait, M., Padgett, M. Y., & Baldwin, T. T. (1989). Job satisfaction and life satisfaction: A reevaluation of the strength of the relationship and gender effects as a function of the date of the study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74(3), 502-507. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.74.3.502 [ Links ]

*Talukder, A. K. M., Vickers, M., & Khan, A. (2018). Supervisor support and work-life balance: Impacts on job performance in the Australian financial sector. Personnel Review, 47(3), 727-744. https://doi.org/10.1108/PR-12-2016-0314 [ Links ]

Taris, T. W., & Schreurs, P. J. G. (2009). Well-being and organizational performance: An organizational-level test of the happy-productive worker hypothesis. Work and Stress, 23(2), 120-136. https://doi.org/10.1080/02678370903072555 [ Links ]

Tellegen, A. (1982). Multidimensional personality questionnaire manual. Unpublished manuscript. Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN. [ Links ]

Tenney, E. R., Poole, J. M., & Diener, E. (2016). Does positivity enhance work performance? Why, when, and what we don’t know. Research in Organizational Behavior, 36, 27-46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.riob.2016.11.002 [ Links ]

Thoresen, C. J., Kaplan, S. A., Barsky, A., Warren, C. R., & de Chermont, K. (2003). The affective underpinnings of job perceptions and attitudes: A meta-analytic review and integration. Psychological Bulletin, 129(6), 914-945. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.6.914 [ Links ]

Tov, W. (2018). Well-being concepts and components. In E. Diener, S. Oishi, & L. Tay (Eds.), Handbook of well-being (pp. 1-15). DEF Publishers. [ Links ]

*Vahle-Hinz, T. (2016). Stress in nonregular work arrangements: A longitudinal study of task and employment-related aspects of stress. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 21(4), 415-431. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039967 [ Links ]

*van Erp, Kim J. P. M., Gevers, J. M. P., Rispens, S., & Demerouti, E. (2018). Empowering public service workers to face bystander conflict: Enhancing resources through a training intervention. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 91(1), 84-109. https://doi.org/10.1111/joop.12190 [ Links ]

Van Rooy, D. L., & Viswesvaran, C. (2004). Emotional intelligence: A meta-analytic investigation of predictive validity and nomological net. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 65(1), 71-95. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-8791(03)00076-9 [ Links ]

Veenhoven, R. (1989). National wealth and individual happiness. In K. G. Grunert, & M. Olander (Eds.), Understanding economic behavior (pp. 1-12). Kluver. [ Links ]

Veenhoven, R. (2018). Subjective well-being in nations. In Diener, E., Oishi, S. & Tay, L. (Eds.), Handbook of well-being. Noba Scholar. [ Links ]

Veenhoven, R. (2019). Happiness in nations. World database of happiness. Erasmus University Rotterdam, The Netherlands. http://worlddatabaseofhappiness.eur.nlLinks ]

Viechtbauer, W., & Cheung, M. W. L. (2010). Outlier and influence diagnostics for meta-analysis. Research Synthesis Methods, 1(2), 112-125. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.11 [ Links ]

*Virick, M., DaSilva, N., & Arrington, K. (2010). Moderators of the curvilinear relation between the extent of telecommuting and job and life satisfaction: The role of performance outcome orientation and worker type. Human Relations, 63(1), 137-154. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018726709349198 [ Links ]

Viswesvaran, C. (2001). Assessment of individual job performance: A review of the past century and a look ahead. In N. Anderson, D. S. Ones, H. K. Sinangil, & C. Viswesvaran (Eds.), Handbook of industrial, work and organizational psychology, Vol 1 (pp. 110-126). Sage. [ Links ]

*Wadhawan, K. (2016). Psychological well-being as a predictor of job performance and job satisfaction. International Journal of Academic Research and Development, 1(3), 1-3. [ Links ]

*Walker, A. G. (2013). The relationship between the integration of faith and work with life and job outcomes. Journal of Business Ethics, 112, 453-461. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-012-1271-0 [ Links ]

Walsh, L. C., Boehm, J. K., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2018). Does happiness promote career success? Revisiting the evidence. Journal of Career Assessment, 26(2), 199-219. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069072717751441 [ Links ]

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063-1070. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063 [ Links ]

Webster, G. D. (2019). Meta-analysis: An introduction. In J. E. Edlund & A. L. Nichols (Eds.), Advanced research methods for the social sciences (pp. 346-367). Cambridge University Press. [ Links ]

Whetzel, D. L., Rotenberry, P. F., & McDaniel, M. A. (2014). In-basket validity: A systematic review. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 22(1), 62-79. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijsa.12057 [ Links ]

Wilcox, R. R. (2014). Introduction to robust estimation and hypothesis testing (3rd ed.). Elsevier. [ Links ]

*Wright, T. A., & Bonett, D. G. (1997). The role of pleasantness and activation-based well-being in performance prediction. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Health Psychology, 2(3), 212-219. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.2.3.212 [ Links ]

*Wright, T. A., & Bonett, D. G. (2007). Job satisfaction and psychological well-being as nonadditive predictors of workplace turnover. Journal of Management, 33(2), 141-160. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206306297582 [ Links ]

*Wright, T. A., Bonett, D. G., & Sweeney, D. A. (1993). Mental health and work performance: Results of a longitudinal field study. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 66(4), 277-284. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8325.1993.tb00539.x [ Links ]

*Wright, T. A., & Cropanzano, R. (2000). Psychological well-being and job satisfaction as predictors of job performance. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5(1), 84-94. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.5.1.84 [ Links ]

*Wright, T. A., Cropanzano, R., & Bonett, D. G. (2007). The moderating role of employee positive well-being on the relation between job satisfaction and job performance. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12(2), 93-104. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.12.2.93 [ Links ]

*Wright, T. A., Cropanzano, R., Denney, P. J., & Moline, G. L. (2002). When a happy worker is a productive worker: A preliminary examination of three models. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 34(3), 146-150. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0087165 [ Links ]

*Wright, T. A., Cropanzano, R., & Meyer, D. G. (2004). State and trait correlates of job performance: A tale of two perspectives. Journal of Business and Psychology, 18(3), 365-383. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JOBU.0000016708.22925.72 [ Links ]

*Wright, T. A., & Hobfoll, S. E. (2004). Commitment, psychological well-being and job performance: An examination of conservation of resources (COR) theory and job burnout. Journal of Business and Management, 9(4), 389-405. [ Links ]

*Wright, T. A., & Staw, B. M. (1999). Affect and favorable work outcomes: Two longitudinal tests on the happy-productive worker thesis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20(1), 1-23. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(199901)20:1<1::AID-JOB885>3.0.CO;2-W [ Links ]

*Youssef, C. M., & Luthans, F. (2007). Positive organizational behavior in the workplace: The impact of hope, optimism, and resilience. Journal of Management, 33(5), 774-800. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206307305562 [ Links ]

*Zelenski, J. M., Murphy, S. A., & Jenkins, D. A. (2008). The happy-productive worker thesis revisited. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9(4), 521-537. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-008-9087-4 [ Links ]

*Zheng, X., Zhu, W., Zhao, H., & Zhang, C. (2015). Employee well-being in organizations: Theoretical model, scale development, and cross-cultural validation. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 36(5), 621-644. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.1990 [ Links ]

Received: November 08, 2021; Accepted: January 12, 2022; pub: February 11, 2022

Correspondence: silvia.moscoso@usc.es (S. Moscoso).

Conflict of Interest

The authors of this article declare no conflict of interest.

Creative Commons License This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial No Derivative License, which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium provided the original work is properly cited and the work is not changed in any way.